Skip to main content

Full text of "Tuning and temperament : a historical survey"

See other formats


Digitized  by  the  Internet  Archive 

in  2011  with  funding  from 

Boston  Library  Consortium  Member  Libraries 


http://www.archive.org/details/tuningtemperamenOObarb 


Tuning  and 
Temperament 

2S 

A 

Historical 
Survey 


J.  Murray  Barbour 


East  Lansing 
Michigan  State  College  Press 

1951 


Copyright  1951 

BY  MICHIGAN  STATE  COLLEGE  PRESS 
East  Lansing 


2741.9 


usic 

L 


LITHOPRINTED  IN  THE  UNITED  STATES  OF  AMERICA  BY 
CUSHING-MALLOY,  INC.,   ANN  ARBOR,  MICHIGAN,   1951 


PREFACE 


This  book  is  based  upon  my  unpublished  Cornell  dissertation, 
Equal  Temperament:  Its  History  from  Ramis  (1482)  to  Rameau 
(1737),  Ithaca,  1932.  As  the  title  indicates,  the  emphasis  in  the 
dissertation  was  upon  individual  writers.  In  the  present  work 
the  emphasis  is  on  the  theories  rather  than  on  their  promulga- 
tors. Since  a  great  many  tuning  systems  are  discussed,  a  sepa- 
rate chapter  is  devoted  to  each  of  the  principal  varieties  of  tun- 
ing, with  subsidiary  divisions  wherever  necessary.  Even  so, 
the  whole  subject  is  so  complex  that  it  seemed  best  that  these 
chapters  be  preceded  by  a  running  account  (with  a  minimum  of 
mathematics)  of  the  entire  history  of  tuning  and  temperament. 
Chapter  I  also  contains  the  principal  account  of  the  Pythagorean 
tuning,  for  it  is  unnecessary  to  spend  a  chapter  upon  a  tuning 
system  that  exists  in  one  form  only. 

Most  technical  terms  will  be  defined  when  they  first  occur, 
as  well  as  in  the  Glossary,  but  a  few  of  these  terms  should  be  de- 
fined immediately.  Of  small  intervals  arising  from  tuning,  the 
comma  is  the  most  familiar.  The  ordinary  (syntonic  or  Ptole- 
maic) comma  is  the  interval  between  a  just  major  third,  with 
ratio  5:4,  and  a  Pythagorean  ditone  or  major  third,  with  ratio 
81:64.  The  ratio  of  the  comma  (the  ratio  of  an  interval  is  ob- 
tained by  dividing  the  ratio  of  the  higher  pitch  by  that  of  the  lower) 
is  81:80. 

The  Pythagorean  (ditonic)  comma  is  the  interval  between  six 
tones,  with  ratio  531441:262144,  and  the  pure  octave,  with  ratio 
2:1.  Thus  its  ratio  is  531441:524288,  which  is  approximately 
74:73.  The  ditonic  comma  is  about  12/11  as  large  as  the  syn- 
tonic comma.  In  general,  when  the  word  comma  is  used  without 
qualification,  the  syntonic  comma  is  meant. 

There  is  necessarily  some  elasticity  in  the  manner  in  which 
the  different  tuning  systems  are  presented  in  the  following  chap- 
ters. Sometimes  a  writer  has  described  the  construction  of  a 
monochord,  a  note  at  a  time.  That  can  be  set  down  easily  in  the 
form  of  ratios.  More  often  he  has  expressed  his  monochord  as 
a  series  of  string-lengths,  with  a  convenient  length  for  the  fun- 
damental. (Except  in  the  immediate  past,  the  use  of  vibration 
numbers,  inversely  proportional  to  the  string-lengths,  has  been 
so  rare  that  it  can  be  ignored.)  Or  he  may  speak  of  there  being 
so  many  pure  fifths,  and  other  fifths  flattened  by  a  fractional 

v 


PREFACE 


part  of  the  comma.  Such  systems  could  be  transformed  into 
equivalent  string -lengths,  but  this  has  not  been  done  in  this  book 
when  the  original  writer  had  not  done  so. 

Systems  with  intervals  altered  by  parts  of  a  comma  can  be 
shown  without  difficulty  in  terms  of  Ellis'  logarithmic  unit  called 
the  cent,  the  hundredth  part  of  an  equally  tempered  semitone,  or 
1/1200  part  of  an- octave.  Since  the  ratio  of  the  octave  is  2:1, 
the  cent  is  21/120°  .  As  a  matter  of  fact,  such  eighteenth  century 
writers  on  temperament  as  Neidhardt  and  Marpurg  had  a  tuning 
unit  very  similar  to  the  cent:  the  twelfth  part  of  the  ditonic 
comma,  which  they  used,  is  2  cents,  thus  making  the  octave  con- 
tain 600  parts  instead  of  1200. 

The  systems  originally  expressed  in  string- lengths  or  ratios 
may  be  translated  into  cents  also,  although  with  greater  difficulty <= 
They  have  been  so  expressed  in  the  tables  of  this  book,  in  the  be- 
lief that  the  cents  representation  is  the  most  convenient  way  of 
affording  comparisons  between  systems.  In  systems  where  it 
was  thought  they  would  help  to  clarify  the  picture,  exponents  have 
been  attached  to  the  names  of  the  notes.  With  this  method,  de- 
vised by  Eitz,  all  notes  joined  by  pure  fifths  have  the  same  ex- 
ponent. Since  the  fundamental  has  a  zero  exponent,  all  the  notes 
of  the  Pythagorean  tuning  have  zero  exponents.  The  exponent  -1 
is  attached  to  notes  a  comma  lower  than  those  with  zero  expo- 
nents, i.e.,  to  those  forming  pure  thirds  above  those  in  the  zero 
series.  Thus  in  just  intonation  the  notes  forming  a  major  third 
would  beC°-E-1,  etc.  Similarly,  notes  that  are  pure  thirds 
lower  than  notes  already  in  the  system  have  exponents  which  are 
greater  by  one  than  those  of  the  higher  notes.  This  use  of  expo- 
nents is  especially  advantageous  in  comparing  various  systems 
of  just  intonation  (see  Chapter  V).  It  may  be  used  also,  with 
-fractional  exponents,  for  the  different  varieties  of  the  meantone 
temperament.  If  the  fifth  C-G,  for  example,  is  tempered  by  1/4 
comma,  these  notes  would  be  labeled  C°  and  G"    . 

A  device  related  to  the  use  of  integral  exponents  lor  the  notes 
in  just  intonation  is  the  arrangement  of  such  notes  to  show  their 

*For  a  discussion  of  methods  of  logarithmic  representation  of  intervals  see 
J.Murray  Barbour,  "Musical  Logarithms,"  Scripta  Mathematica,  VII  (1940), 
21-31. 


VI 


PREFACE 


harmonic  relationships.  Here,  all  notes  that  are  related  by  fifths, 
i.e.,  that  have  the  same  exponent,  lie  on  the  same  horizontal  line, 
while  their  pure  major  thirds  lie  in  a  parallel  line  above  them, 
each  forming  a  45°  angle  with  the  related  note  below.  Since  the 
pure  minor  thirds  below  the  original  notes  are  lower  by  a  fifth 
than  the  major  thirds  above  them,  they  will  lie  in  the  same  higher 
line,  but  will  form  135°  angles  with  the  original  notes.  For  ex- 
ample: 

A-1     „F-1     „R-1 
C  G 

This  arrangement  is  especially  good  for  showing  extensions  of 
just  intonation  with  more  than  twelve  notes  in  the  octave,  and  it 
is  used  for  that  purpose  only  in  this  book  (see  Chapter  VI). 

It  is  desirable  to  have  some  method  of  evaluating  the  various 
tuning  systems.  Since  equal  temperament  is  the  ideal  system  of 
twelve  notes  if  modulations  are  to  be  made  freely  to  every  key, 
the  semitone  of  equal  temperament,  100  cents,  is  taken  as  the 
ideal,  from  which  the  deviation  of  each  semitone,  as  C-C  ,  C*-D, 
D-E",  etc.,  is  calculated  in  cents.  These  deviations  are  then 
added  and  the  sum  divided  by  twelve  to  find  the  mean  deviation 
(M.D.)  in  cents.  The  standard  deviation  (S.D.)  is  found  in  the 
usual  manner,  by  taking  the  root-mean-square. 

It  should  be  added  that  there  may  be  criteria  for  excellence 
in  a  tuning  system  other  than  its  closeness  to  equal  temperament. 
For  example,  if  no  notes  beyond  E"  or  G^  are  used  in  the  music 
to  be  performed  and  if  the  greatest  consonance  is  desired  for  the 
notes  that  are  used,  then  probably  the  1/5  comma  variety  of  mean- 
tone  temperament  would  be  the  ideal,  since  its  fifths  and  thirds 
are  altered  equally,  the  fifths  being  1/5  comma  flat  and  its  thirds 
1/5  comma  sharp.  If  keys  beyond  two  flats  or  three  sharps  are 
to  be  touched  upon  occasionally,  but  if  it  is  considered  desirable 
to  have  the  greatest  consonance  in  the  key  of  C  and  the  least  in 
the  key  of  G",  then  our  Temperament  by  Regularly  Varied  Fifths 
would  be  the  best.  This  is  a  matter  that  is  discussed  in  detail 
at  the  end  of  Chapter  VTI,  but  it  should  be  mentioned  now. 


My  interest  in  temperament  dates  from  the  time  in  Berlin 
when  Professor  Curt  Sachs  showed  me  his  copy  of  Mersenne's 

vii 


PREFACE 


Harmonie  universelle.  I  am  indebted  to  Professor  Otto  Kinkeldey , 
my  major  professor  at  Cornell,  and  to  the  Misses  Barbara  Dun- 
can and  Elizabeth  Schmitter  of  the  Sibley  Musical  Library  of  the 
Eastman  School  of  Music,  for  assistance  rendered  during  my 
work  on  the  dissertation.  Most  of  my  more  recent  research  has 
been  at  the  Library  of  Congress.  Dr.  Harold  Spivacke  and  Mr. 
Edward  N.  Waters  of  the  Music  Division  there  deserve  especial 
thanks  for  encouraging  me  to  write  this  book.  I  want  also  to 
thank  the  following  men  for  performing  so  well  the  task  of  read- 
ing the  manuscript:  Professor  Charles  Warren  Fox,  Eastman 
School  of  Music;  Professor  Bonnie  M.  Stewart,  Michigan  State 
College;  Dr.  Arnold  Small,  San  Diego  Navy  Electronics  Labora- 
tory; and  Professor  Glen  Haydon,  University  of  North  Carolina. 

J.  Murray  Barbour 


East  Lansing,  Michigan 
November,  1950 


Vlll 


GLOSSARY 


Arithmetical  Division  —  The  equal  division  of  the  difference  be- 
tween  two  quantities,  so  that  the  resultant  forms  an  arithme- 
tical progression,  as  9:8:7:6. 

Bonded  Clavichord  —  A  clavichord  upon  which  two  or  more  con- 
secutive semitones  were  produced  upon  a  single  string. 

Cent  —  The  unit  of  interval  measure.    The  hundredth  part  of  an 

1200i 

equal  semitone,  with  ratio    <y2. 

Circle  of  Fifths  —  The  arrangement  of  the  notes  of  a  closed  sys- 
tem by  fifths,  as  C,  G,  D,  A,  E,  etc. 

Circulating  Temperaments  —  Temperaments  in  which  all  keys 
are  playable,  but  in  which  keys  with  few  sharps  or  flats  are 
favored. 

Closed  System  —  A  regular  temperament  in  which  the  initial  note 
is  eventually  reached  again. 

Column  of  Differences  —  See  Tabular  Differences. 

Comma  —  A  tuning  error,  such  as  the  interval  B^-C  in  the  Py- 
thagorean tuning.    See  Ditonic  Comma  and  Syntonic  Comma. 

Ditone  —  A  major  third,  especially  one  formed  by  two  equal  tones, 
as  L?  the  Pythagorean  tuning  (81:64). 

Ditonic  Comma  —  The  interval  between  two  enharmonically 
equivalent  notes,  as  B  and  C,  in  the  Pythagorean  tuning.  Its 
ratio  is  531441:524288  or  approximately  74:73,  and  it  is  con- 
ventionally taken  as  24  cents. 

Duplication  of  the  Cube  —  A  problem  of  antiquity,  equivalent  to 
finding  two  geometrical  means  between  two  quantities  one  of 
which  is  twice  as  large  as  the  other,  or  to  finding  the  cube  root 
of  2. 

Exponents  —  In  tuning  theory  exponents  are  used  to  indicate  de- 
viations from  the  Pythagorean  tuning,  the  unit  being  the  syn- 
tonic comma.  Plus  values  are  sharper  and  minus  values  flatter 
than  the  corresponding  Pythagorean  notes.  Fractional  expo- 
nents indicate  subdivisions  of  the  comma,  as  in  the  meantone 
and  many  irregular  temperaments. 

ix 


GLOSSARY 


Equal  Temperament  —  The  division  of  the  octave  into  an  equal 
number  of  parts,  specifically  into  12  semitones,  each  of  which 
has  the  ratio  of     >f2. 

Euclidean  Construction  —  Euclid's  method  for  finding  a  mean 
proportional  between  two  lines,  by  describing  a  semicircle 
upon  the  sum  of  the  lines  taken  as  a  diameter  and  then  erecting 
a  perpendicular  at  the  juncture  of  the  two  lines. 

Fretted  Clavichord  —  See  Bonded  Clavichord. 

Fretted  Instruments  —  Such  modern  instruments  as  the  guitar 
and  banjo,  or  the  earlier  lute  and  viol. 

Generalized  Keyboard  —  A  keyboard  arranged  conveniently  for 
the  performance  of  multiple  divisions. 

Geometrical  Division  —  The  proportional  division  of  two  quanti- 
ties, so  that  the  resultant  forms  a  geometrical  progression, 
as  27:18:12:8. 

Golden  System  —  A  system  of  tuning  based  on  the  ratio  of  the 
golden  section   (  /f5~ -  1):2. 

Good  Temperaments  —  See  Circulating  Temperaments. 

Irregular  System  —  Any  tuning  system  with  more  than  one  odd- 
sized  fifth,  with  the  exception  of  just  intonation. 

Just—  Pure:  A  term  applied  to  intervals,  as  the  just  major  third. 

Just  Intonation  —  A  system  of  tuning  based  on  the  octave  (2:1), 
the  pure  fifth  (3:2),  and  the  pure  major  third  (5:4). 

Linear  Correction  —  The  arithmetical  division  of  the  error  in  a 

string-length. 
Mean-Semitone  Temperament  —  A   temperament   in   which  the 

diatonic  notes  are  in  meantone  temperament,  and  the  chromatic 

notes  are  taken  as  halves  of  meantones. 

Meantone  Temperament  —  Strictly,  a  system  of  tuning  with  flat- 
tened  fifths  (y  5:1)  and  pure  major  thirds  (5:4).  See  Varieties 
of  Meantone  Temperament. 


x 


GLOSSARY 


Meride  —  Sauveur's  tuning  unit,  1/43  octave,  that  is,  ^"27  Each 
meride  was  divisible  into  7  eptamerides,  and  each  of  the  ep- 
tamerides  into  10  decamerides. 

Mesolabium  —  An  instrument  of  the  ancients  for  finding  mechan- 
ically 2  mean  proportionals  between  2  given  lines.  See  illus- 
tration, p.  51. 

Monochord  —  A  string  stretched  over  a  wooden  base  upon  which 
are  indicated  the  string-lengths  for  some  tuning  system;  a 
diagram  containing  these  lengths;  directions  for  constructing 
such  a  diagram. 

Monopipe  —  A  variable  open  pipe,  with  indicated  lengths  for  a 
scale  in  a  particular  tuning  system,  thus  fulfilling  a  function 
similar  to  that  of  a  monochord. 

Multiple  Division  —  The  division  of  the  octave  into  more  than  12 
parts,  equal  or  unequal. 

Negative  System  —  A  regular  system  whose  fifth  has  a  ratio 
smaller  than  3:2. 

Positive  System  —  A  regular  system  whose  fifth  has  a  ratio 
larger  than  3:2. 

Ptolemaic  Comma  —  See  Syntonic  Comma. 

Pythagorean  Comma  —  See  Ditonic  Comma. 

Pythagorean  Tuning  —  A  system  of  tuning  based  on  the  octave 
(2:1)  and  the  pure  fifth  (3:2). 

Regular  Temperament  —  A  temperament  in  which  all  the  fifths 
save  one  are  of  the  same  size,  such  as  the  Pythagorean  tuning 
or  the  meantone  temperament.  (Equal  temperament,  with  all 
fifths  equal,  is  also  a  regular  temperament,  and  so  are  the 
closed  systems  of  multiple  division.) 

Schisma  —  The  difference  between  the  syntonic  and  ditonic 
commas,  with  ratio  32805:32768,  or  approximately  2  cents. 

Semi-Meantone  Temperament  —  See  Mean-Semitone  Tempera- 
ment. 


XI 


GLOSSARY 


Sesqui The  prefix  used  to  designate  a  superparticular  ratio, 

as  sesquitertia  (4:3). 

Sexagesimal  Notation  —  The  use  of  60  rather  than  10  as  a  base 
of  numeration,  as  in  the  measurement  of  angles. 

Split  Keys  —  Separate  keys  on  a  keyboard  instrument  for  such  a 
pair  of  notes  as  G*  and  Ab. 

String- Length  —  The  portion  of  a  string  on  the  monochord  that 
will  produce  a  desired  pitch. 

Subsemitonia  —  See  Split  Keys. 

Superparticular  Ratio  —  A  ratio  in  which  the  antecedent  exceeds 
the  consequent  by  1,  as  5:4.    See  Sesqui-. 

Syntonic  Comma  —  The  interval  between  a  just  major  third  (5:4) 
and  a  Pythagorean  third  (81:64).  Its  ratio  is  81:80  and  it  is 
conventionally  taken  as  22  cents. 

Tabular  Differences  -  The  differences  between  the  successive 
terms  in  a  sequence  of  numbers,  such  as  a  geometrical  pro- 
gression. 

TemPer  -  To  vary  the  pitch  slightly.  A  tempered  fifth  is  spe- 
cifically a  flattened  fifth. 

Temperament  —  A  system,  some  or  all  of  whose  intervals  can- 
not be  expressed  in  rational  numbers. 

A  Tuning  -  A  system  all  of  whose  intervals  can  be  expressed  in 
rational  numbers. 

Tuning  Pipe  —  See  Monopipe., 

Unequal  Temperament  —  Any  temperament  other  than  equal 
temperament,  particularly  the  meantone  temperament  or  some 
variety  thereof. 

Varieties  of  Meantone  Temperament  —  Regular  temperaments 
formed  on  the  same  principle  as  the  meantone  temperament, 
with  flattened  fifths  and  (usually)  sharp  thirds. 

Wolf  Fifth  -  The  dissonant  fifth,  usually  G#-Eb  (notated  as  a 
diminished  sixth),  in  any  unequal  temperament,  such  as  the 
meantone  v/olf  fifth  of  737  cents. 

xii 


CONTENTS 


Preface Page  v 

Glossary ix 

I.  History  of  Tuning  and  Temperament 1 

II.  Greek  Tunings 15 

III.  Meantone  Temperament 25 

Other  Varieties  of  Meantone  Temperament  ....  31 

IV.  Equal  Temperament 45 

Geometrical  and  Mechanical  Approximations  ...  49 

Numerical  Approximations 55 

V.  Just  Intonation 89 

Theory  of  Just  Intonation 102 

VI.  Multiple  Division 107 

Equal  Divisions 113 

Theory  of  Multiple  Division 128 

VII.  Irregular  Systems 133 

Modifications  of  Regular  Temperaments 139 

Temperaments  Largely  Pythagorean 151 

Divisions  of  Ditonic  Comma 156 

Metius'  System 177 

"Good"  Temperaments 178 

VIII.  From  Theory  to  Practice 185 

Tuning  of  Keyboard  Instruments 188 

Just  Intonation  in  Choral  Music 196 

Present  Practice 199 

Literature  Cited 203 

Index    219 

xiii 


CONTENTS 


LIST  OF  ILLUSTRATIONS 

Frontispiece:    Fludd's  Monochord,  with  Pythagorean 
Tuning  and  Associated  Symbolism 

Fig.  A.    Schneegass'  Division  of  the  Monochord 38 

B.  The  Mesolabium 51 

C.  Roberval's  Method  for  Finding  Two  Geo- 

metric Mean  Proportionals 52 

D.  Nicomedes'     Method     for     Finding    Two 

Geometric  Mean  Proportionals 53 

E.  Strahle's  Geometrical  Approximation  for 

Equal  Temperament 66 

F.  Gibelius'  Tuning  Pipe 86 

G.  Mersenne's    Keyboard    with    Thirty-One 

Notes  in  the  Octave 109 

H.    Ganassi's   Method   for    Placing  Frets  on 

the  Lute  and  Viol 142 

I.    Bermudo's  Method  for  Placing  Frets  on 

the  Vihuela 165 


Chapter  I.    HISTORY  OF  TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


The  tuning  of  musical  instruments  is  as  ancient  as  the  musical 
scale.  In  fact,  it  is  much  older  than  the  scale  as  we  ordinarily 
understand  it.  If  primitive  man  played  upon  an  equally  primitive 
instrument  only  two  different  pitches,  these  would  represent  an 
interval  of  some  sort  —  a  major,  minor,  or  neutral  third;  some 
variety  of  fourth  or  fifth;  a  pure  or  impure  octave.  Perhaps  his 
concern  was  not  with  interval  as  such,  but  with  the  spacing  of 
soundholes  on  a  flute  or  oboe,  the  varied  lengths  of  the  strings 
on  a  lyre  or  harp.  Sufficient  studies  have  been  made  of  extant 
specimens  of  the  wind  instruments  of  the  ancients,  and  of  all 
types  of  instruments  used  by  primitive  peoples  of  today,  for 
scholars  to  come  forward  with  interesting  hypotheses  regarding 
scale  systems  anterior  to  our  own.  So  far  there  has  been  no 
general  agreement  as  to  whether  primitive  man  arrived  at  an 
instrumental  scale  by  following  one  or  another  principle,  several 
principles  simultaneously,  or  no  principle  at  all.  Since  this  is 
the  case,  there  is  little  to  be  gained  by  starting  our  study  prior 
to  the  time  of  Pythagoras,  whose  system  of  tuning  has  had  so 
profound  an  influence  upon  both  the  ancient  and  the  modern  world. 

The  Pythagorean  system  is  based  upon  the  octave  and  the 
fifth,  the  first  two  intervals  of  the  harmonic  series.  Using  the 
ratios  of  2:1  for  the  octave  and  3:2  for  the  fifth,  it  is  possible  to 
tune  all  the  notes  of  the  diatonic  scale  in  a  succession  of  fifths 
and  octaves,  or,  for  that  matter,  all  the  notes  of  the  chromatic 
scale.  Thus  a  simple,  but  rigid,  mathematical  principle  under- 
lies the  Pythagorean  tuning.  As  we  shall  see  in  the  more  detailed 
account  of  Greek  tunings,  the  Pythagorean  tuning  perse  was  used 
only  for  the  diatonic  genus,  and  was  modified  in  the  chromatic 
and  enharmonic  genera.  In  this  tuning  the  major  thirds  are  a 
ditonic  comma  (about  1/9  tone)  sharper  than  the  pure  thirds  of 
the  harmonic  series.  When  the  Pythagorean  tuning  is  extended 
to  more  than  twelve  notes  in  the  octave,  a  sharped  note,  as  G#, 
is  higher  than  the  synonymous  flatted  note,  as  A". 

The  next  great  figure  in  tuning  history  was  Aristoxenus,  whose 
dispute  with  the  disciples  of  Pythagoras  raised  a  question  that  is 
eternally  new:    are  the  cogitations  of  theorists  as  important  as 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


the  observations  of  musicians  themselves?  His  specific  conten- 
tion was  that  the  judgment  of  the  ear  with  regard  to  intervals 
was  superior  to  mathematical  ratios.  And  so  we  find  him  talking 
about  "parts'7  of  an  octave  rather  than  about  string-lengths „  One 
of  Aristoxenus7  scales  was  composed  of  equal  tones  and  equal 
halves  of  tones.  Therefore  Aristoxenus  was  hailed  by  sixteenth 
century  theorists  as  the  inventor  of  equal  temperament.  How- 
ever, he  may  have  intended  this  for  the  Pythagorean  tuning,  for 
most  of  the  other  scales  he  has  expressed  in  this  unusual  way 
correspond  closely  to  the  tunings  of  his  contemporaries.  From 
this  we  gather  that  his  protest  was  not  against  current  practice, 
but  rather  against  the  rigidity  of  the  mathematical  theories. 

Claudius  Ptolemy,  the  geographer,  is  the  third  great  figure  in 
early  tuning  history.  To  him  we  are  in  debt  for  an  excellent 
principle  in  tuning  lore:  that  tuning  is  best  for  which  ear  and 
ratio  are  in  agreement.  He  has  made  the  assumption  here  that 
it  is  possible  to  reach  an  agreement.  The  many  bitter  arguments 
between  the  mathematicians  and  the  plain  musicians,  even  to  our 
own  day,  are  evidence  that  this  agreement  is  not  easily  obtained, 
but  may  actually  be  the  result  of  compromise  on  both  sides.  To 
Ptolemy  the  matter  was  much  simpler.  For  him  a  tuning  was 
correct  if  itused  superparticular  ratios,  such  as  5:4,  11:10,  etc. 
All  of  the  tuning  varieties  which  he  advocated  himself  are  con- 
structed exclusively  with  such  ratios.  To  us,  nearly  2000  years 
later,  his  tunings  seem  as  arbitrary  as  was  that  of  Pythagoras. 

Ptolemy's  syntonic  diatonic  has  especial  importance  to  the 
modern  world  because  it  coincides  with  just  intonation,  a  tuning 
system  founded  on  the  first  five  intervals  of  the  harmonic  series 
—  octave,  fifth,  fourth,  major  third,  minor  third.  Didymus'  dia- 
tonic used  the  same  intervals,  but  in  slightly  different  order.  If 
it  could  be  shown  that  Ptolemy  favored  his  syntonic  tuning  above 
any  of  the  others  which  he  has  presented,  the  adherents  of  just 
intonation  from  the  sixteenth  century  to  the  twentieth  century 
would  be  on  more  solid  ground  in  hailing  him  as  their  patron 
saint.  Actually  he  approved  the  syntonic  tuning  because  its  ratios 
are  superparticular;  but  so  are  the  ratios  of  three  of  the  four 
other  diatonic  scales  he  has  given. 

Just  intonation,  in  either  the  Ptolemy  or  the  Didymus  ver- 


HISTORY  OF  TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


sion,  was  unknown  throughout  the  Middle  Ages.  Boethius  dis- 
cussed all  three  of  the  above-mentioned  authorities  on  tuning, 
but  gave  in  mathematical  detail  only  the  system  of  Pythagoras. 
It  was  satisfactory  for  the  unisonal  Gregorian  chant,  for  its  small 
semitones  are  excellent  for  melody  and  its  sharp  major  thirds 
are  no  drawback.  Even  when  the  first  crude  attempt  at  harmony 
resulted  in  the  parallel  fourths  and  fifths  of  organum,the  Pytha- 
gorean tuning  easily  held  its  own. 

But,  later,  thirds  and  sixths  were  freely  used  and  were  con- 
sidered imperfect  consonances  rather  than  dissonances.  It  has 
been  questioned  whether  these  thirds  and  sixths  were  as  rough  as 
they  would  have  been  in  the  strict  Pythagorean  tuning,  or  whether 
a  process  of  softening  (tempering)  had  not  already  begun.  At 
least  one  man,  the  Englishman  Walter  Odington,  had  stated  that 
consonant  thirds  had  ratios  of  5:4  and  6:5,  and  that  singers  intui- 
tively used  these  ratios  instead  of  those  given  by  the  Pythagorean 
monochord.  In  reply  one  might  note  that  some  theorists  continued 
to  advocate  the  Pythagorean  tuning  for  centuries  after  the  com- 
mon practice  had  become  something  quite  different.  If  it  was 
good  enough  for  them,  surrounded  as  they  were  by  other,  less 
harsh,  tuning  methods,  it  must  have  sufficed  for  most  of  those 
who  lived  in  an  age  when  no  other  definite  system  of  tuning  was 
known. 

The  later  history  of  the  Pythagorean  tuning  makes  interesting 
reading.*  It  was  still  strongly  advocated  in  the  early  sixteenth 
century  by  such  men  as  Gafurius  and  Ornithoparchus,  and  formed 
the  basis  for  the  excellent  modification  made  by  Grammateus 
and  Bermudo.  At  the  end  of  the  century  Papius  spoke  in  its  favor, 
and  so,  forty  years  later,  did  Robert  Fludd.  In  the  second  half 
of  the  seventeenth  century  Bishop  Caramuel,  who  has  the  inven- 
tion of  "musical  logarithms"  to  his  credit,  said  that  "very  many" 
(plurimi)  of  his  contemporaries  still  followed  in  the  footsteps  of 
Pythagoras.  Like  testimony  was  given  half  a  century  later  from 
England,  where  Malcolm  wrote  that  "some  and  even  the  Generality 
. . .  tune  not  only  their  Octaves,  but  also  their  5ths  as  perfectly  . . . 
Concordant  as  their  Ear  can  judge,  and  consequently  make  their 

!See  J.  Murray  Barbour,"  The  Persistence  of  the  Pythagorean  Tuning  Sys- 
tem," Scripta  Mathematica,  I  (1933),  286-304. 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


4ths  perfect,  which  indeed  makes  a  great  many  Errors  in  the  other 
Intervals  of  3rd  and  6th."  After  another  half  century  we  find 
Abbe  Roussier  extolling  "triple  progression,"  as  he  called  the 
Pythagorean  tuning,  and  praising  the  Chinese  for  continuing  to 
tune  by  perfect  fifths. 

Like  the  systems  of  Agricola  in  the  sixteenth  century  and  of 
Dowland  in  the  early  seventeenth  century,  many  of  the  numerous 
irregular  systems  of  the  eighteenth  century  contained  more  pure 
than  impure  fifths.  The  instruments  of  the  violin  family,  tuned 
by  fifths,  have  a  strong  tendency  toward  the  Pythagorean  tuning. 
And  a  succession  of  roots  moving  by  fifths  is  the  basis  of  our 
classic  system  of  harmony  from  Rameau  to  Prout  and  Goetschius. 
Truly  the  Pythagorean  tuning  system  has  been  long-lived,  and  is 
still  hale  and  hearty! 

To  return  to  the  fifteenth  century  and  the  dissatisfied  per- 
formers: Almost  certainly  some  men  did  dislike  the  too-sharp 
major  thirds  and  the  too-flat  minor  thirds  so  much  that  they  at- 
tempted to  improve  them.  But  history  has  preserved  no  record 
of  their  experiments.  And  the  vast  majority  must  have  still  been 
using  the  Pythagorean  system,  with  all  its  imperfections,  when 
Ramis  de  Pareja  presented  his  tuning  system  to  the  world. 

To  be  sure,  Ramis  did  not  present  himself  as  the  champion 
of  a  tremendous  innovation.  He  was  not  a  Luther  nailing  his 
ninety-five  theses  to  the  church  door.  His  tuning  was  offered  as 
a  method  which  would  be  easier  to  work  out  on  the  monochord, 
and  thus  would  be  of  greater  utilitarian  value  to  the  singer,  than 
was  the  Pythagorean  tuning,  with  its  cumbersome  ratios.  Al- 
though Ramis'  monochord  contained  four  pure  thirds,  with  ratio 
5:4,  it  was  not  the  usual  form  of  just  intonation  applied  to  the 
chromatic  octave,  in  which  eight  thirds  will  be  pure.  It  is  rather 
to  be  considered  an  irregular  tuning,  combining  features  of  both 
the  Pythagorean  tuning  and  just  intonation.  Some  of  Ramis'  con- 
temporaries assailed  his  tuning  method,  but  his  pupil  Spataro 
explained  it  as  a  sort  of  temperament  of  the  Pythagorean  tuning. 
From  these  polemics  arose  the  entirely  false  notion  that  Ramis 
was  an  advocate  of  equal  temperament.  ^   But  he  is  worthy  of  our 

2lt  occurs,  for  example,  in  such  a  general  work  as  Sir  James  Jeans'  Science 
and  Music  (New  York,  1937). 


HISTORY  OF  TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


respect  as  the  first  of  a  long  line  of  innovators  and  reformers  in 
the  field  of  tuning. 

As  the  words  "tuning"  and  "temperament"  aye  used  today,  the 
former  is  applied  to  such  systems  as  the  Pythagorean  and  just, 
in  which  all  intervals  may  be  expressed  as  the  ratio  of  two  in- 
tegers. Thus  for  any  tuning  it  is  possible  to  obtain  a  monochord 
in  which  every  string-length  is  an  integer.  A  temperament  is  a 
modification  of  a  tuning,  and  needs  radical  numbers  to  express 
the  ratios  of  some  or  all  of  its  intervals.  Therefore,  in  mono- 
chords  for  temperaments  the  numbers  given  for  certain  (or  all) 
string-lengths  are  only  approximations,  carried  out  to  a  partic- 
ular degree  of  accuracy.  Actually  it  is  difficult  in  extreme  cases 
to  distinguish  between  tunings  and  temperaments.  For  example, 
Bermudo  constructed  a  monochord  in  which  the  tritone  G-C# 
has  the  ratio  164025:115921.  This  differs  by  only  1/7  per  cent 
from  the  tritone  of  equal  temperament,  and  in  practice  could  not 
have  been  differentiated  from  it.  But  his  system,  which  consists 
solely  of  linear  divisions,  should  be  called  a  tuning  rather  than 
a  temperament. 

It  is  not  definitely  known  when  the  practice  of  temperament 
first  arose  in  connection  with  instruments  of  fixed  pitch,  such  as 
organs  and  claviers.  Even  in  tuning  an  organ  by  Pythagorean 
fifths  and  octaves,  the  result  would  not  be  wholly  accurate  if  the 
tuner's  method  was  to  obtain  unisons  between  pitches  on  a  mono- 
chord  and  the  organ  pipes.  This  would  be  a  sort  of  unconscious 
temperament.  More  consciously  he  may  have  tried  to  improve 
some  of  the  harsh  Pythagorean  thirds  by  lopping  a  bit  off  one 
note  or  another.  Undoubtedly  this  was  being  done  during  the 
fifteenth  century,  for  we  find  Gafurius,  at  the  end  of  that  century, 
mentioning  that  organists  assert  that  fifths  undergo  a  small  dimi- 
nution called  temperament  (participata).^ 

We  have  no  way  of  knowing  what  temperament  was  like  in 
Gafurius'  age;  but  it  is  my  belief  that  this  diminution  which  Ga- 
furius characterized  as  "minimae  ac  latentis  incertaeque  quo- 
demmodo  quantitatis"  was  actually  so  small  that  organs  so  tuned 
came  closer  to  being  in  equal  temperament  than  in  just  intonation 

3Franchinus  Gafurius,  Practica  musica  (Milan,  1496),  Book  2,  Chapter  3. 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


or  the  meantone  temperament.  This  belief  is  substantiated  by 
two  German  methods  of  organ  temperament  which  appeared  in 
print  a  score  of  jgears  later  than  Gafurius'  tome.  The  earlier  of 
the  two  was  Arnold  Schlick's  temperament,  an  irregular  method 
for  which  his  directions  were  somewhat  vague,  but  in  which  there 
were  ten  flattened  and  two  raised  fifths,  as  well  as  twelve  raised 
thirds.  Shohe  Tanaka's  description  of  Schlick's  method4  as  the 
meantone  temperament  is  wholly  false;  for  in  the  latter  the  eight 
usable  thirds  are  pure.  Actually,  from  Schlick's  own  account, 
the  method  lay  somewhere  between  the  meantone  temperament 
and  the  equal  temperament.  More  definite  and  certainly  very 
near  to  equal  temperament  was  Grammateus'  method,  in  which 
the  white  keys  were  in  the  Pythagorean  tuning  and  the  black  keys 
were  precisely  halfway  between  the  pairs  of  adjoining  white  keys. 

Just  what  the  players  themselves  at  this  time  understood  by 
equal  semitones  is  not  known.  Perhaps  they  would  have  been 
satisfied  with  a  tuning  like  that  of  Grammateus,  with  ten  semi- 
tones equal  and  the  other  two  smaller.  The  first  precise  math- 
ematical definition  of  equal  temperament  was  given  by  Salinas: 
"We  judge  this  one  thing  must  be  observed  by  makers  of  viols, 
so  that  the  placing  of  the  frets  may  be  made  regular,  namely  that 
the  octave  must  be  divided  into  twelve  parts  equally  proportional, 
which  twelve  will  be  the  equal  semitones. "5  To  facilitate  con- 
structing this  temperament  on  the  monochord,  Salinas  advised 
the  use  of  the  mesolabium,  a  mechanical  method  for  finding  two 
mean  proportionals  between  two  given  lines .  Zarlino  also  gave 
mechanical  and  geometric  methods  for  finding  the  mean  propor- 
tionals, intended  primarily  for  the  lute.  (Zarlino  did  include, 
however,  Ruscelli's  enthusiastic  plea  that  all  instruments,  even 
organs,  should  be  tuned  equally.)  The  history  of  equal  tempera- 
ment, then,  is  chiefly  the  history  of  its  adoption  upon  keyboard 
instruments. 


4aStudien  im  Gebiete  der  reinen  Stimmung,"  Vierteljahrsschrift  fiir  Musik- 
wissenschaft,  VI  (1890),  62,  63. 

5Francisco  Salinas,  De  musica  libri  VH  (Salamanca,  1577),  p.  173. 
6 


HISTORY  OF  TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Neither  Salinas  nor  Zarlino  gave  monochord  lengths  for  equal 
temperament,  although  the  problem  was  not  extremely  difficult: 
to  obtain  the  12th  root  of  2,  take  the  square  root  twice  and  then 
the  cube  root.  The  first  known  appearance  in  print  of  the  correct 
figures  for  equal  temperament  was  in  China,  where  Prince  Tsai- 
yii's  brilliant  solution  remains  an  enigma,  since  the  music  of  China 
had  no  need  for  any  sort  of  temperament.  More  significant  for 
European  music,  but  buried  in  manuscript  for  nearly  three  cen- 
turies, was  Stevin's  solution.  As  important  as  this  achievement 
was  his  contention  that  equal  temperament  was  the  only  logical 
system  for  tuning  instruments,  including  keyboard  instruments. 
His  contemporaries  apologetically  presented  the  equal  system  as 
a  practical  necessity,  but  Stevin  held  that  its  ratios,  irrational 
though  they  may  be,  were  "true"  and  that  the  simple,  rational 
values  such  as  3:2  for  the  fifth  were  the  approximations!  In  his 
day  only  a  mathematician  (and  perhaps  only  a  mathematician  not 
fully  cognizant  of  contemporary  musical  practice)  could  have 
made  such  a  statement.  It  is  refreshingly  modern,  agreeing 
completely  with  the  views  of  Schbnberg  and  other  advanced  theo- 
rists and  composers  of  our  day. 

The  most  complete  and  important  discussion  of  tuning  and 
temperament  occurs  in  the  works  of  Mersenne.  There,  in  addition 
to  his  valuable  contributions  to  acoustics  and  his  descriptions  of 
instruments,  Mersenne  ran  the  whole  gamut  of  tuning  theory.  He 
expressed  equal  temperament  in  numbers,  indicated  geometrical 
and  mechanical  solutions  for  it,  and  finally  put  it  upon  the  prac- 
tical basis  of  tuning  by  beats  as  used  today.  Fully  as  catholic  is 
his  list  of  instrumental  groups  for  which  this  temperament  should 
be  used:  all  fretted  instruments,  all  wind  instruments,  all  key- 
board instruments,  and  even  percussion  instruments  (bells)." 
The  widespread  influence  of  Mersenne's  greatest  work,  Harmonie 
universelle  (Paris,  1636  -  37),  undoubtedly  helped  greatly  to 
popularize  a  tuning  that  was  then  still  considered  as  suitable  for 
lutes  and  viols  only. 

The  first  really  practical  approximation  for  equal  tempera - 

Gjohann  Philip  Albrecht  Fischer,  Verhandlung  van  de  Klokken  en  het  Klokke- 
Spel  (Utrecht,  1738),  p.  19,  gave  a  bell  temperament,  with  C  equal  to  192.000. 
This  was  equal  temperament,  with  a  few  minor  errors. 

7 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


ment  had  been  presented  by  Vincenzo  Galilei  half  a  century  before 
Mersenne.  He  showed  that  the  ratio  of  18:17  was  convenient  in 
fretting  the  lute.  Since  references  to  this  size  of  semitone  cover 
two  and  a  half  centuries,  it  is  probable  that  it  has  been  used  even 
longer  by  makers  of  lutes,  guitars,  and  the  like.  Of  course  the 
repeated  use  of  the  18:17  ratio  would  not  give  an  absolutely  pure 
octave,  but  a  slight  adjustment  in  the  intervals  would  correct  the 
error.  Galilei's  explanation  of  the  reason  for  equal  semitones 
on  the  lute  is  logical  and  correct:  Since  the  frets  are  placed 
straight  across  the  six  strings,  the  order  of  diatonic  and  chro- 
matic semitones  is  the  same  on  all  strings.  Hence,  in  playing 
chords,  C*  might  be  sounded  on  one  string  and  DD  on  another, 
and  this  will  be  a  very  false  octave  unless  the  instrument  is  in 
equal  temperament. 

Vicentino  had  referred  to  a  serious  difficulty  that  arose  from 
the  common  practice  of  having  one  kind  of  tuning  (meantone)  for 
keyboard  instruments  and  another  (equal)  for  fretted  instruments. 
Since  the  pitches  were  so  divergent,  there  was  dissonance  when- 
ever the  two  groups  were  used  together.  By  1600,  theorists  like 
Artusi  and  Bottrigari  said  that  these  different  groups  of  instru- 
ments were  not  used  simultaneously  because  of  the  pitch  diffi- 
culties. That  is  why  such  large  instrumental  groups  were  needed 
as  those  employed  in  the  Ballet  Comique  de  la  Reine  or  in  Mon- 
teverdi's Orfeo  —  selected  groups  of  like  instruments  sounded 
well,  but  the  mixture  of  different  tunings  made  tuttis  impracti- 
cable. It  would  seem  that  this  consideration  would  have  brought 
about  the  universal  adoption  of  equal  temperament  long  before  it 
did  come.  However,  after  the  unfretted  violins  became  the  back- 
bone of  the  seventeenth  century  orchestra,  their  flexibility  of  in- 
tonation made  this  problem  less  pressing  than  when  lutes  and 
viols  had  been  opposed  to  organs  and  claviers. 

Before  we  leave  the  sixteenth  century,  we  should  examine  the 
contribution  to  tuning  history  for  which  Vicentino  is  especially 
known.  His  archicembalo  was  an  instrument  with  six  keyboards, 
with  a  total  of  thirty-one  different  pitches  in  the  octave.  He  de- 
scribed its  tuning  as  that  of  the  "usage  and  tuning  common  to  all 
the  keyboard  instruments,  as  organs,  cembali,  clavichords,  and 


HISTORY  OF  TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


the  like.  "^  This  would  have  been  the  ordinary  meantone  temper- 
ament, in  which  the  fifths  were  tempered  by  1/4  comma.  Huy- 
ghens,  a  century  and  a  half  after  Vicentino,  showed  that  there  was 
very  close  correspondence  between  a  system  in  which  the  octave 
is  divided  into  thirty-one  logarithmically  equal  parts  and  the 
meantone  system,  similarly  extended  to  thirty- one  parts. 

A  simpler  type  of  multiple  division  was  the  cembalo  with 
nineteen  notes  in  the  octave.  Both  Zarlino  and  Salinas  intended 
their  variants  of  the  meantone  temperament  (with  fifths  tempered 
by  2/7  and  by  1/3  comma  respectively)  for  such  an  instrument, 
and  the  latter 's  temperament  would  result  in  an  almost  precisely 
equal  division.  Praetorius  described  such  an  instrument  also, 
and  it  has  received  favor  with  some  twentieth  century  writers, 
especially  Yasser. 

The  best  system  of  multiple  division  within  the  limits  of  prac- 
ticability divides  the  octave  into  fifty-three  parts.  This  is  lit- 
erally a  scale  of  commas,  and,  as  such,  was  suggested  by  the 
ancient  Greek  writers  on  the  Pythagorean  system.  Mersenne 
and  Kircher  in  the  seventeenth  century  mentioned  the  system. 
Mercator  realized  its  advantages  for  measuring  intervals.  But 
especial  honor  should  be  paid  to  the  nineteenth  century  English- 
man Bosanquet  for  devising  an  harmonium  with  a  "generalized 
keyboard"  upon  which  the  53-system  could  be  performed. 

Other  varieties  of  equal  multiple  division  will  be  discussed 
in  Chapter  VI,  together  with  a  number  of  unequal  divisions,  most 
of  which  are  extensions  of  just  intonation.  Practical  musicians 
have  rejected  all  of  them,  chiefly  because  they  are  more  difficult 
to  play,  as  well  as  being  more  expensive,  than  our  ordinary  key- 
boards. 

Just  intonation,  as  has  already  been  mentioned,  has  had  few 
devotees  since  the  early  seventeenth  century.  The  history  of  the 
meantone  temperament  makes  more  interesting  reading,  since 
various  theorists  in  addition  to  Zarlino  and  Salinas  had  conflict- 
ing ideas  as  to  the  amount  by  which  the  fifths  should  be  tempered. 
Silbermann's  temperament  of  1/6  comma  for  the  fifths  is  the 
most  significant  for  us,  because  he  represents  the  more  con- 

^Nicola  Vicentino,  L'antica  musica  ridotta  alia  moderna  prattica  (Rome,  1555), 
Book  5,  Chapter  6. 

9 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


servative  practice  during  the  time  of  Bach  and  Handel.  In  his 
temperament  the  thirds  are  slightly  sharp,  but  the  wolves  are 
almost  as  ravenous  as  in  the  Aron  1/4  comma  system. 

To  some  extent  the  final  adoption  of  equal  temperament  for 
an  individual  organ  or  clavier  might  have  meant  substituting  this 
temperament  for  some  type  of  meantone  temperament.  We  are 
told  that  organs  in  England  were  still  generally  in  meantone  tem- 
perament until  the  middle  of  the  nineteenth  century.  England 
must  have  lagged  behind  the  Continent  in  this  respect,  and  it  is 
quite  possible  that  the  change,  when  it  did  come,  was  radical. 

But  it  is  more  likely  that  in  most  cases  the  change  to  equal 
temperament  was  made  more  smoothly  than  this.  The  importance 
of  unequal  systems  of  twelve  notes  to  the  octave  has  been  gen- 
erally neglected  by  the  casual  historians  of  tuning,  to  whom  only 
the  Big  Four  (Pythagorean,  just,  meantone,  and  equal)  are  of 
moment.  It  is  my  opinion,  however,  that  the  unequal  systems 
were  of  the  greatest  possible  significance  in  bringing  about  the 
supremacy  of  our  present  tuning  system.  Reference  has  already 
been  made  to  the  early  sixteenth  century  irregular  systems  of 
Schlick  and  Grammateus.  The  former  resembled  the  meantone 
temperament;  the  latter  was  derived  from  the  Pythagorean  tuning. 
Bermudo  repeated  Grammateus'  tuning,  and  his  own  second 
method  was  basically  Pythagorean  also.  Ramis  and  Agricola 
crossed  just  intonation  with  the  Pythagorean  tuning,  with  fairly 
happy  issue.  Ganassi  and  Artusi  treated  just  intonation  and  the 
meantone  temperament  much  as  Grammateus  and  Bermudo  had 
treated  the  Pythagorean  tuning. 

Only  a  few  years  later  than  Grammateus,  Aron  described  for 
organs  the  meantone  temperament,  mentioned  above.  In  it  every 
fifth  save  one  was  tempered  by  such  an  amount  (1/4  comma,  or 
about  1/18  semitone)  that  four  fifths  less  two  octaves  would  pro- 
duce a  pure  major  third.  Thus  arose  the  system  that,  with  var- 
ious modifications,  was  to  be  the  strongest  opponent  of  equal 
temperament,  so  far  as  keyboard  instruments  were  concerned, 
for  two  or  three  hundred  years.  In  the  meantone  temperament  a 
sharped  note,  as  G%  is  lower  in  pitch  than  the  equivalent  flattened 
note,  as  AD,  by  the  great  diesis,  which  is  almost  half  as  large 
as  a  semitone. 


10 


HISTORY  OF  TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


After  Aron's  time  the  meantone  temperament,  or  some  similar 
system,  was  generally  accepted  for  organ  and  clavier.  But  there 
were  a  few  dissenting  voices.  One  was  that  of  his  exact  contem- 
porary Lanfranco,  whose  practical  tuning  rules  for  keyboard  in- 
struments seem  to  agree  with  no  system  other  than  equal  tem- 
perament. Another  was  that  of  Fogliano,  who  was  apparently  the 
first  sixteenth  century  writer  to  follow  Ramis'  lead  and  use  in  a 
tuning  system  both  the  pure  fifths  and  the  pure  thirds  of  just  in- 
tonation. But  there  is  a  difference;  for  he  realized  that  the  triads 
on  D  and  BD  would  be  hopelessly  out  of  tune  in  such  a  system, 
and  therefore  recommended  that  there  be  a  mean  D  and  Bb,  each 
differing  by  half  a  comma  from  a  pair  of  just  pitches.  These  two 
mean  pitches  hint  at  Aron's  meantone  system.  Otherwise  this  is 
what  we  ordinarily  understand  just  intonation  to  be.  Ironically 
enough,  Fogliano's  method,  although  containing  more  perfect 
thirds  than  Ramis'  did,  is  far  inferior  to  it  if  one  goes  beyond  the 
ordinary  bounds  of  two  flats  and  three  sharps.  Beyond  these 
bounds  lay  in  wait  the  howling  wolves,  to  muffle  whose  voices 
was  the  task  of  many  a  later  worker  in  this  field. 

Fogliano  had  no  immediate  followers  as  an  advocate  of  just 
intonation,  since  the  following  generation  was  more  concerned 
with  temperament.  But  almost  a  century  later,  certain  mathe- 
maticians —  as  Galileo,  de  Caus,and  Kepler  —  proclaimed  again 
the  validity  of  pure  thirds  and  fifths.  Occasional  lone  figures, 
both  mathematicians  and  music  theorists,  were  to  speak  in  favor 
of  just  intonation,  even  until  our  own  day.  But  it  is  significant 
that  the  great  music  theorists,  such  as  Zarlino,  Mersenne,  and 
Rameau,  presented  just  intonation  as  the  theoretical  basis  of  the 
scale,  but  temperament  as  a  practical  necessity.  Equally  great 
mathematicians  with  some  understanding  of  music,  from  Stevin 
to  Max  Planck,  have  hailed  temperament. 

From  the  middle  of  the  sixteenth  century,  all  the  theorists 
agreed  that  the  fretted  instruments,  lutes  and  viols,  were  tuned 
in  equal  temperament.  Vicentino  made  the  first  known  reference 
to  this  fact,  going  so  far  as  to  state  that  both  types  of  instrument 
had  been  so  tuned  from  their  invention.  If  we  may  believe  pic- 
torial evidence,  especially  that  of  the  Flemish  painters,  so  me- 
ticulous about  detail,  frets  were  adjusted  to  equal  temperament 

11 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


as  early  as  1500,  although  there  is  not  complete  agreement  on 
this  point. 

In  the  National  Gallery  in  London,  for  example,  there  are 
several  paintings  in  which  the  position  of  frets  is  shown  plainly. 
A  Concert,  by  Ercole  de  Roberti  (1450-96),  contains  a  nine- 
stringed  lute  and  a  small  four-stringed  viol,  both  apparently  in 
equal  temperament.  Marco  Marziale's  Madonna  and  Child  En- 
throned with  Saints,  painted  between  1492  and  1507,  has  an  eleven- 
stringed  lute  with  intervals  equally  proportional.  And  The  Am- 
bassadors, painted  by  Hans  Holbein  the  Younger  in  1533,  has  a 
six-stringed  lute,  again  in  equal  temperament.  Negative  evidence 
is  furnished  by  a  painting  by  the  early  sixteenth  century  painter 
Ambrogio  de  Predis,  whose  Angel  Playing  on  a  Musical  Instru- 
ment is  playing  a  nine- stringed  lute  on  which  the  semitones  run 
large,  small,  small,  large,  and  then  three  equal,  as  if  the  notes 
might  have  been  C,  C*,  D,  E  ,  E,  etc. 

Because  of  the  ease  of  tuning  perfect  fifths,  the  Pythagorean 
tuning  has  been  the  foundation  of  many  of  the  later  irregular  sys- 
tems, including  that  of  Kirnberger.  It  also  had  some  importance 
for  such  sophisticated  writers  as  Werckmeister,  Neidhardt,  and 
Marpurg,  whose  systems  with  subtly  divided  commas  were  di- 
rected to  the  intellect  rather  than  to  the  ear  of  the  practical  mu- 
sician. 

It  becomes  apparent,  however,  from  the  works  of  the  men  just 
mentioned  that  an  instrument  that  was  "well  tempered"  was  not 
necessarily  tempered  equally.  The  title  of  Bach's  famous  "48" 
meant  simply  that  the  clavier  was  playable  in  all  keys.  Werck- 
meister and  Neidhardt  explained  clearly  that  in  their  systems 
the  key  of  C  would  be  the  best  and  D*3  the  worst,  with  the  conso- 
nance of  the  other  keys  somewhere  between  these  extremes. 

Mersenne's  and  Rameau's  modification  of  the  1/4  comma 
meantone  temperament  resembles  somewhat  the  "good"  temper- 
aments of  Werckmeister  and  Neidhardt,  and  Gallimard,  with  the 
aid  of  logarithms,  reached  a  very  similar  goal.  Perhaps  the  best 
of  these  many  irregular  systems  was  Thomas  Young's  second 
method,  in  which  six  fifths  are  perfect,  and  the  other  six  are  tuned 


12 


HISTORY  OF  TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


flat  by  1/6  Pythagorean  comma,  as  in  Silbermann's  tuning.  This 
would  have  been  simpler  to  construct  by  ear  than  most  of  the 
systems,  and  does  have  an  orderly  progression  from  good  to  poor 
tuning  as  one  departs  from  the  most  common  keys. 

In  almost  all  of  these  irregular  systems,  from  Grammateus 
to  Young,  all  the  major  thirds  were  sharp  to  some  extent,  thus 
differing  from  just  intonation  and  the  meantone  temperament,  in 
which  the  usable  thirds  were  perfect  and  the  others  very  harsh. 
For  the  practical  musician  it  would  have  been  an  easy  matter,  as 
time  went  on,  to  tune  the  "common"  thirds  still  sharper,  so  that 
all  the  thirds  would  be  equally  sharp,  and  his  instrument  would 
be  substantially  in  equal  temperament.  Probably  this  is  exactly 
what  did  happen. 

The  recorded  opposition  to  equal  temperament  on  the  part  of 
such  men  as  Werckmeister  and  even  Sebastian  Bach  was  to  the 
rigorous  mathematical  treatment  implied  by  the  name  "gleich- 
schwebend."  Theirs  was  a  practical  approximation  to  equality, 
and,  from  the  keyboard  compositions  of  Bach,  it  is  evident  that 
his  practice  must  have  been  as  satisfactory  as  that  of  our  present- 
day  tuners,  else  the  great  majority  of  his  compositions  would 
have  been  unbearable. 


13 


Chapter  II.    GREEK  TUNINGS 


Greek  music  theory  is  highly  complex  and  difficult,  with  its  al- 
phabetical notation,  the  dependence  of  musical  rhythm  upon  poetic 
meter,  and  all  the  rest  of  it.  Our  confusion  is  not  lessened  by 
the  fact  that  scholars  quarrel  about  the  exact  interpretation  of 
the  modal  scales  and  that  a  pitifully  scant  remnant  of  the  music 
itself  is  available  for  study  today.  Fortunately  it  is  possible  to 
understand  the  essentials  of  Greek  tuning  theories  without  enter- 
ing into  the  other  and  more  controversial  aspects  of  Greek  mu- 
sical science.  Moreover,  it  is  advisable  that  the  Greek  tuning 
lore  be  presented  in  some  detail  in  order  that  the  attitude  of  many 
sixteenth  and  seventeenth  century  theorists  may  be  clarified. 

The  foundation  of  the  Greek  scale  was  the  tetrachord,  a  de- 
scending series  of  four  notes  in  the  compass  of  the  modern  per- 
fect fourth.  Most  typical  was  the  Dorian  tetrachord,  with  two 
tones  and  then  a  semitone,  as  A  G  F  E  or  E  D  C  B.  Two  or  more 
tetrachords  could  be  combined  by  conjunction,  as  the  above  tetra- 
chords  would  be  with  E  a  common  note.  Or  they  might  be  com- 
bined by  disjunction,  as  the  above  tetrachords  would  be  in  reverse 
order,  with  a  whole  tone  between  B  and  A.  Tetrachords  combined 
alternately  by  conjunction  and  by  disjunction  correspond  to  our 
natural  heptatonic  scale. 

The  Greeks  had  three  genera— diatonic,  chromatic,  and  en- 
harmonic. A  diatonic  tetrachord  contained  two  tones  and  a  semi- 
tone, variously  arranged,  the  Dorian  tetrachord  having  the  order 
shown  above,  as  A  G  F  E.  In  the  chromatic  tetrachord  the  second 
string  (as  G)  was  lowered  until  the  two  lower  intervals  in  the 
tetrachord  were  equal.  Thus  A  G"  F  E  represents  the  process 
of  formation  better  than  the  more  commonly  shown  A  F*  F  E. 
In  the  enharmonic  tetrachord  the  second  string  was  lowered  still 
further  until  it  was  in  unison  with  the  third  string;  the  third  string 
was  then  tuned  half  way  between  the  second  and  fourth  strings. 
In  notes  the  enharmonic  tetrachord  would  be  A  G""  F  E  or  A  F 
F  E.  Thus  in  the  chromatic  tetrachord  there  were  the  consecu- 
tive semitones  that  we  associate  with  the  modern  chromatic 
genus;  but  the  enharmonic  tetrachord  contained  real  quarter 
tones,  whereas  our  enharmonically  equivalent  notes,  as  F*5  and 
E,  differ  by  a  comma,  1/9  tone,  or  at  most  by  a  diesis,  1/5  tone. 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Claudius  Ptolemy  has  presented  the  most  complete  list  of 
tunings  advocated  by  various  theorists,  including  himself. * 
These  (with  one  exception  to  be  discussed  later)  were  shown  by 
the  ratios  of  the  three  consecutive  intervals  that  constituted  the 
tetrachord,  and  also  by  string- lengths  for  the  octave  lying  be- 
tween 120  and  60,  using  sexagesimal  fractions  where  necessary. 
The  octave  is  the  Dorian  octave,  as  from  E  to  E,  with  B-A  the 
disjunctive  tone,  always  with  9:8  ratio.  Ptolemy's  tables  are 
given  here  (Tables  1-21)  with  comments  following.  The  frac- 
tions have  been  changed  into  decimal  notation. 


Greek  Enharmonic  Tunings 


Table  1.    Archytas'  Enharmonic 

Lengths 

60.00      75.00      77.14      80.00      90.00      112.50       115.71       120.00 

Names 

ECCBA                F                F                E 

Ratios 

5/4         36/35       28/27       9/8         5/4           36/35        28/27 

Cents         1200         814         765         702         498  112  63 


Table  2.    Aristoxenus'  Enharmonic 

Lengths     60.00      76.00      78.00      80.00     90.00      114.00  117.00      120.00 

b  b 

Names           E             C             C             B             A               F  F                E 

Parts                  16            2             2            10           24              3  3 

Cents          1200         791          746         702         498             89  44                 0 


Table  3.    Eratosthenes'  Enharmonic 

Lengths     60.00      75.00      77.50      80.00     90.00      112.50       116.25       120.00 

b  b 

Names  ECCBA  F  F  E 

Ratios  5/4         24/23      46/45       9/8         5/4  24/23        46/45 

Cents         1200         814         740         702         498  112  38  0 

*Claudii   Ptolemaei   Harmonicorum    libri   tres.     Latin   translation   by    John 
Wallis  (London,  1699). 

16 


GREEK  TUNINGS 


Greek  Chromatic  Tunings 


Table  4.    Archytas'  Chromatic 

Lengths  60.00      71.11       77.14      80.00      90.00      106.67       115.71       120.00 

Names  E           Db            C             B             A             Gb               F                E 

Ratios  32/27     243/224    28/27     9/8      32/27      243/224       28/27 

Cents  1200         906         765         702         498          204            63                0 

Table  5.    Aristoxenus'  Chromatic  Malakon 

Lengths     60.00      74.67      77.33  80.00     90.00      112.00  116.00  120.00 

Db             C  B             A             Gb  F                E 

23"          23  10           22              4  4 

821         761  702         498         119  59                0 

Table  6.    Aristoxenus'  Chromatic  Hemiolion 

Lengths  60.00     74.00  77.00  80.00     90.00     111.00      115.50      120.00 

Names  E           Db  C            B            A             Gb              F               E 

Parts  14            3  3              10           21               42             4* 

Cents  1200         837  768  702         498         135             66                0 

Table  7.    Aristoxenus'  Chromatic  Tonikon 

Lengths  60.00     72.00  76.00  80.00  90.00     108.00  114.00      120.00 

Names  E           Db  C            B  A  Gb  F               E 

Parts  12           4  4            10  18  6  6 

Cents  1200         884  791  702  498         182  89               0 


Names 

E 

Parts 

14  3 

Cents 

1200 

17 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  8.    Eratosthenes'  Chromatic 

Lengths  60.00      72.00       76.00      80.00      90.00      108.00       114.00      120.00 
Names  E  Db  C  B  A  Gb  F  E 

Ratios  6/5        19/18       20/19      9/8         6/5  19/18         20/19 

Cents  1200         884  791  702         498         182  89  0 

Table  9.    Didymus'  Chromatic 

Lengths  60.00      72.00       75.00      80.00      90.00      108.00       112.50       120.00 
Names  E  Db  C  B  A  Gb  F  E 

Ratios  6/5         25/24       16/15      9/8         6/5  25/24  16/15 

Cents  1200         884         814  702         498  182  112  0 

Table  10.    Ptolemy's  Chromatic  Malakon 

Lengths  60.00      72.00       77.14      80.00      90.00      108.00       115.71       120.00 
Names  E  Db  C  B  A  Gb  F  E 

Ratios  6/5         15/14       28/27      9/8         6/5  15/14  28/27 

Cents  1200         884         765         702         498         182  63  0 

Table  11.    Ptolemy's  Chromatic  Syntonon 

Lengths  60.00      70.00       76.36      80.00      90.00      105.00       114.55       120.00 
Names  E  Db  C  B  A  Gb  F  E 

Ratios  7/6         12/11       22/21       9/8         7/6  12/11  22/21 

Cents  1200         933         783  702         498         231  81  0 


18 


GREEK  TUNINGS 


Greek  Diatonic  Tunings 


Table  12.    Archytas'  Diatonic 

Lengths  60.00      67.50       77.14      80.00      90.00      101.25       115.71       120.00 
Names  EDCBAG  F  E 

Ratios  9/8  8/7         28/27      9/8         9/8  8/7  28/27 

Cents  1200         996         765         702         498  294  63  0 

Table  13.    Aristoxenus'  Diatonic  Malakon 

Lengths  60.00      70.00      76.00      80.00      90.00      105.00  114.00       120.00 

Names           EDCBAG  F                E 

Parts  10  6  4  10  15  9  6 

Cents  1200         933         791          702         498         231  89                 0 

Table  14.    Aristoxenus'  Diatonic  Syntonon 

Lengths  60.00      68.00      76.00      80.00      90.00      102.00       114.00  120.00 

Names  EDCBAG                F                E 

Parts  8             8             4            10           12              12               6 

Cents  1200         983         791          702         498         281              89                 0 

Table  15.    Eratosthenes'  Diatonic 

Lengths  60.00      67.50      75.94      80.00      90.00      101.25       113.91       120.00 

Names  EDCBAG                F                 E 

Ratios  9/8          9/8      256/243     9/8         9/8           9/8       256/243 

Cents  1200         996         792         702         498         294              90                 0 


19 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  16.    Didymus'  Diatonic 

Lengths  60.00      67.50       75.00      80.00      90.00      101.25       112.50       120.00 
Names  EDCBAG  F  E 

Ratios  9/8  10/9       16/15      9/8         9/8  10/9  16/15 

Cents  1200         996         814         702         498  294  112  0 

Table  17.    Ptolemy's  Diatonic  Malakon 

Lengths  60.00      68.57       76.19      80.00      90.00      102.86       114.27       120.00 
Names  EDCBAG  F  E 

Ratios  8/7         10/9       21/20      9/8         8/7  10/9  21/20 

Cents  1200         969  787         702         498         265  85  0 

Table  18.    Ptolemy's  Diatonic  Toniaion 

Lengths  60.00      67.30       77.14      80.00      90.00      101.25       115.71       120.00 
Names  EDCBA  G  F  E 

Ratios  9/8         8/7       28/27       9/8         9/8  8/7  28/27 

Cents  1200         996         765         702         498         294  63  0 

Table  19.    Ptolemy's  Diatonic  Ditoniaion 

Lengths  60.00      67.50       75.94      80.00      90.00      101.25       113.91       120.00 

Names  EDCBA               G               F                E 

Ratios  9/8         9/8     256/243      9/8         9/8           9/8         256/243 

Cents  1200         996         792         702         498         294              90                 0 

Table  20.    Ptolemy's  Diatonic  Syntonon 

Lengths  60.00      66.67       75.00      80.00      90.00      100.00      112.50       120.00 

Names  EDCBAG                F                E 

Ratios  10/9        9/8        16/15       9/8        10/9         9/8           16/15 

Cents  1200       1018         814          702         498         316           112                  0 

20 


GREEK  TUNINGS 


Table  21.    Ptolemy's  Diatonic  Hemiolon 

Lengths  60.00      66.67       73.33      80.00      90.00      100.00       110.00       120.00 
Names  EDCBA  G  F  E 

Ratios  10/9        11/10       12/11     9/8  10/9         11/10         12/11 

Cents  1200       1018         853         702         498         316  151  0 


Only  two  of  these  seventeen  or  eighteen  independent  tunings 
have  had  any  great  influence  upon  modern  music  theory— the  third 
and  fourth  of  Ptolemy's  diatonic  scales,  commonly  called  the 
"ditonic"  and  the  "syntonic."  The  former  is  the  same  as  Era- 
tosthenes' diatonic,  and  is  the  old  Pythagorean  tuning.  It  gains 
its  name  from  the  fact  that  its  major  third  (ditone)  consists  of  a 
pair  of  equal  tones.  The  latter,  the  "tightly  stretched"  in  con- 
trast to  the  "soft"  (malakon),  is  what  we  know  as  just  intonation. 
Didymus' diatonic  contains  the  same  intervals  as  Ptolemy's  syn- 
tonic diatonic,  but  with  the  minor  tone  (10:9)  below  the  major  tone 
(9:8)  instead  of  the  reverse.  Didymus'  arrangement  is  the  more 
logical  for  constructing  amonochord;  Ptolemy's  in  terms  of  the 
harmonic  series. 

The  theorists  of  the  sixteenth  and  seventeenth  centuries, 
eager  to  bolster  their  ideas  with  classical  prototypes,  pointed 
out  that  the  just  tuning  was  that  of  Didymus  and  Ptolemy.  But 
they  ignored  the  other  diatonic  tunings  of  Ptolemy.  They  liked 
to  point  out  further  that  in  three  of  the  enharmonic  tunings  the 
pure  major  third  (5:4)  appears,  and  in  four  of  the  chromatic 
tunings  the  pure  minor  third  (6:5).  But  only  Didymus  used  en- 
harmonic and  chromatic  tunings  that  really  resembled  just  into- 
nation. His  chromatic  is  tuned  precisely  as  E,  C*,  C,  etc.,  would 
be  in  just  intonation,  using  the  chromatic  semitone,  25:24,  which 
appears  in  no  other  tuning.  In  his  enharmonic,  not  only  does  the 
major  third  have  the  ratio  5:4,  but  the  small  intervals  are  "equal" 
quarter  tones,  resulting  from  an  arithmetical  division  of  the 
16:15  semitone.*  The  other  nine  enharmonic  and  chromatic  tun- 
ings depart  more  or  less  from  Didymus'  standard. 


21 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Let  us  examine  more  of  the  peculiarities  of  these  Greek  tun- 
ings. Archytas  has  used  the  same  ratio  (28:27)  for  the  lowest 
interval  in  each  genus,  thus  having  an  interval  (63  cents)  that  is 
much  larger  than  most  of  the  semitones  and  smaller  than  the 
quarter  tones.  The  ditonic  semitone,  256:243,  is  about  the  same 
size  as  Ptolemy's  "soft"  semitone,  21:20,  being  a  comma  smaller 
than  the  syntonic  semitone,  16:15.  The  tones  range  from  mini- 
mum, 11:10,  through  minor,  10:9,  and  major,  9:8,  to  maximum, 
8:7.  Archytas'  minor  third,  32:27,  is  a  comma  larger  than  the 
syntonic  third,  6:5,  and  more  than  a  comma  smaller  than  Ptol- 
emy's minor  third,  7:6.  Eratosthenes'  major  third,  19:15,  is 
about  the  same  size  as  the  Pythagorean  ditone,  81:64,  and  is  about 
a  ditonic  comma  larger  than  the  syntonic  third,  5:4. 

Ever  since  his  own  age  a  great  controversy  has  raged  about 
the  teachings  of  Aristoxenus.  Instead  of  using  ratios,  he  divided 
the  tetrachord  into  30  parts,  of  which,  in  his  diatonic  syntonon, 
each  tone  has  12  parts,  each  semitone  6.  Thus,  if  we  are  to  take 
him  at  his  word,  Aristoxenus  was  here  describing  equal  tem- 
perament. The  sixteenth  and  seventeenth  century  theorists  were 
of  the  opinion  that  such  was  his  intention,  the  advocates  of  equal 
temperament  opposing  the  name  of  Aristoxenus  to  that  of  Ptolemy. 

Ptolemy  himself  did  not  so  understand  Aristoxenus' doctrines. 
With  a  fundamental  of  120  units,  the  perfect  fourth  above  has  90 
units.  Thus,  as  shown  in  the  tables,  Ptolemy  subtracted  Aris- 
toxenus' "parts"  from  120.  His  enharmonic  then  agrees  with  that 
of  Eratosthenes,  and  his  chromatic  tonikon  with  the  latter 's 
chromatic.  But  Aristoxenus'  diatonic  syntonon  does  not  then 
quite  agree  with  the  Pythagorean  (ditonic)  diatonic,  although  the 
latter  is  the  only  Greek  tuning  that  contains  two  equal  tones. 
His  diatonic  malakon,  as  Ptolemy  has  shown  it,  is  unlike  any  of 
the  other  tunings;  whereas  in  its  succession  of  intervals— large, 
medium,  small  —  it  resembles  Ptolemy's  diatonic  malakon  or 
chromatic  syntonon. 

So  it  seems  quite  likely  that  Aristoxenus  did  not  intend  to  ex- 
press any  new  tunings  by  his  adding  together  of  parts  of  a  tone, 
but  simply  to  indicate  in  a  general  way  the  impression  that  cur- 
rent tunings  made  upon  the  ear.  But  his  vagueness  has  made 
possible  all  sorts  of  wild  speculations.    It  is  even  possible,  by 


22 


GREEK  TUNINGS 


an  improper  manipulation  of  the  figures,  to  argue  that  Aris- 
toxenuswas  a  proponent  of  just  intonation.  Take  his  enharmonic: 
24  +  3  +  3.  Add  these  numbers  to  90  in  reverse  order  as  before, 
getting  90  93  96  120.  Then  consider  these  numbers  to  be  fre- 
quencies rather  than  string- lengths.  The  result  is  practically 
thesameasDidymus':  5/4  x  32/31  x  31/30.  Or  take  Aristoxenus' 
diatonic  syntonon:  12  +  12  +  6.  Treat  it  as  we  have  just  treated 
his  enharmonic,  getting  90  96  108  120.  If  these  are  then  taken 
as  frequencies,  we  have  Ptolemy's  syntonic,  10/9  x  9/8  x  16/15. 

The  paramount  principle  in  Ptolemy's  tunings  was  the  use  of 
superparticular  proportion,  a  ratio  in  which  the  antecedent  ex- 
ceeds the  consequent  by  unity.  (The  Latin  prefix  "sesqui"  is 
conveniently  used  to  describe  these  ratios,  e.g.,  "sesquiquarta," 
meaning  5/4.)  Ptolemy  used  5/4,  6/5,  7/6,  8/7,  etc.  Seven  of 
the  eight  tunings  that  bear  his  own  name  are  constructed  entirely 
of  superparticular  proportions,  the  eighth  being  the  ditonic,  or 
Pythagorean.  Seven  tunings  that  he  has  ascribed  to  other  writers 
also  use  these  ratios  exclusively,  including  all  of  Didymus'  tun- 
ings, Archytas'  enharmonic  and  diatonic,  and  Eratosthenes'  chro- 
matic (Aristoxenus'  chromatic  tonikon).  In  just  intonation  the 
ratios  are,  of  course,  superparticular,  and  this  feature  only 
would  have  appealed  to  Ptolemy  and  his  contemporaries.  For, 
despite  the  many  apparently  just  intervals  used  in  the  given  tun- 
ings, Ptolemy  recognized  no  consonances  other  than  those  of  the 
Pythagorean  tuning— fourth,  fifth,  octave,  eleventh,  twelfth,  and 
fifteenth. 

It  is  easy  to  obtain,  by  algebra,  all  the  possible  divisions  of 
the  tetrachord  built  up  entirely  by  superparticular  proportions. 
(A  theory  for  the  superparticular  division  of  tones  is  shown  in 
connection  with  Colonna,  in  Chapter  VIL)  Eliminating  those  in 
which  one  interval  is  considerably  smaller  than  the  smallest 
enharmonic  quarter  tone  (46:45),  we  find  that,  collectively,  the 
Greeks  had  not  omitted  many  possibilities.  Other  enharmonic 
tunings  similar  to  Ptolemy's  would  be  5/4  x  22/21  x  56/55  and 
5/4  x  26/25  x  40/39.  Chromatic  tunings  would  include  6/5  x 
13/12  x  40/39;  7/6  x  9/8  x  64/63;  7/6  x  10/9  x  36/35;  and  7/6 x 
15/14  x  16/15.  Two  others  are  difficult  to  classify:  8/7  x  13/12 
x  14/13  might  best  be  considered  a  chromatic  tuning,  something 


23 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


like  14  +  8  +  8  in  Aristoxenus'  parts.  And  8/7  x  8/7  x  49/48  is 
undoubtedly  a  variant  of  the  ditonic  tuning,  but  with  a  quarter  tone 
instead  of  a  semitone  at  the  bottom,  perhaps  14  +  14  +  2. 

In  later  chapters  we  shall  see  many  echoes  of  Greek  tuning 
methods,  not  only  in  such  well-known  systems  as  the  Pythagorean 
and  the  just,  but  also  in  the  modified  systems,  such  as  Ganassi's, 
and  in  irregular  systems,  such  as  Dowland's.  Unusual  super- 
particular  intervals  are  used  by  Colonna  in  the  poorest  tuning 
system  shown  in  this  book,  and  also  by  Awraamoff ,  whose  system 
is  even  worse. 


24 


Chapter  III.    MEANTONE  TEMPERAMENT 


It  is  not  definitely  known  when  temperament  was  first  used. 
Vicentino  stated  that  the  fretted  instruments  had  always  been 
in  equal  temperament.  As  for  the  keyboard  instruments,  Zar- 
lino  declared  that  temperament  was  as  old  as  the  complete 
chromatic  keyboard.  It  may  well  be  that  some  organs  in  the 
fifteenth  century  had  had  temperament  of  a  sort,  although  the 
Pythagorean  tuning  continued  to  have  too  many  advocates  not  to 
have  been  dominant  in  the  earlier  period.  However  that  may 
be,  Riemann  discovered  the  first  mention  of  temperament  in  a 
passage  from  Gafurius'  Practica  musica  (1496). 1  There,  among 
the  eight  rules  of  counterpoint,  Gafurius  said  that  organists  as- 
sert that  fifths  undergo  a  small,  indefinite  amount  of  diminution 
called  temperament  (participata).  Since  he  was  reporting  a  con- 
temporary fact,  rather  than  advocating  an  innovation,  the  practice 
may  have  begun  decades  earlier  than  his  time. 

Notice  that  Gafurius  stated  that  there  was  nothing  regular 
about  the  temperament  of  his  day,  nor  were  the  fifths  diminished 
by  any  large  amount.  It  seems  reasonable  to  believe  that  when 
organists  first  became  dissatisfied  with  the  extremely  sharp 
thirds  of  the  Pythagorean  tuning,  they  would  go  about  any  altera- 
tion of  the  fifths  in  a  gingerly  manner,  lopping  off  a  bit  here  and 
a  bit  there.  Grammateus'  division  of  Pythagorean  tones  into 
equal  semitones  came  only  twenty-two  years  after  Gafurius' 
observation, 2  and  ranks  very  high  among  irregular  systems  that 
approach  equal  temperament.  It  is  easy  to  believe,  therefore, 
that  organs  were  tuned  as  well  in  1500  as  they  generally  are 
today. 

Dechales  had  no  authority  for  stating  that  Guido  of  Arezzo 
was  the  father  of  temperament. «*  The  association  of  Ramis^  with 


^ugo  Riemann,  Geschichte  der  Musiktheorie  (Berlin,  1898),  p.  327. 
2See  Chapter  VII  for  Grammateus. 

3R.  P.  Claudius  Franciscus    Milliet  Dechales,  Cursus  seu  mundus  mathe- 
maticus  (Lugduni,  1674),  Tomus  Tertius,  pp.  15-17. 

^See  Preface  and  Chapter  V. 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


temperament  is  one  of  the  most  common  misconceptions  in  the 
history  of  tuning.  And,  although  Schlick's  system^  undoubtedly 
can  properly  be  described  as  a  temperament,  it  is  just  as  surely 
of  an  irregular  variety.  It  is  well  to  mention  these  names,  and 
discard  each  of  them,  before  saying  that  full  credit  for  describ- 
ing the  meantone  temperament  must  go  to  Pietro  Aron. 

InAron's  Toscanello^  there  is  a  chapter  entitled  "Concerning 
the  temperament  (participation)  and  way  of  tuning  the  instrument. " 
The  tuning  is  to  be  made  in  three  successive  stages  (see  Table 
22).  First,  the  major  third,  C-E,  is  to  be  made  "sonorous  and 
just."  But  the  fifth  C-G  is  to  be  made  "a  little  flat."  The  fifth 
G-D  is  to  be  similarly  flattened,  and  then  A  is  to  be  tuned  so  that 
the  fifths  D-A  and  A-E  are  equal.  The  idea,  of  course,  is  to  en- 
sure an  equality  of  these  four  fifths,  so  far  as  it  can  be  accom- 
plished by  ear. 


Table  22.    Aron's  Meantone  Temperament  (1/4  Comma) 

7  1.31+1  3  1  2  3+1  5 

Names  C°  C#"  D"  Eb  <  E-  F  *  F*"  G-*  G#"  A"  Bb  *  B"  C° 
Cents   0  76   193  310  386  503  579  697  773  890  1007  1083  1200 
M.D.  20.0;  S.D.  20.2 


In  the  second  stage  of  tuning,  the  fifths  F-C,  B^-F,  and 
E^-B0  are  tempered  exactly  the  same  as  the  diatonic  fifths  had 
been.  Finally,  in  the  third  stage,  C^  and  F#  are  tuned  as  pure 
thirds  to  A  and  D  respectively.  Aron  says  nothing  about  G^. 
With  Kinkeldey  we  can  say  that  this  note  "probably  belongs  to  the 
third  group,  "7  and  would  be  tuned  as  a  pure  third  to  E. 

The  name  "meantone"  was  applied  to  this  temperament  be- 
cause the  tone,  as  C-D,  is  precisely  half  of  the  pure  third,  as 


5See  Chapter  VII. 

^Toscanello  in  musica  (Venice,  1523);  revised  edition  of  1529  was  consulted.. 

?Otto  Kinkeldey,  Orgel  und  Klavier  in  der  Musik  des  16.   Jahrhunderts  (Leip- 
zig, 1910),  p.  76. 

26 


MEANTONE  TEMPERAMENT 


C-E.  Aron  said  nothing  about  the  division  of  the  comma.  But 
since  the  pure  E  is  a  syntonic  comma  lower  than  the  Pythagorean 
E,and  each  fifth  is  to  be  tempered  by  the  same  amount,  the  fifths 
will  all  be  tempered  by  1/4  comma.  It  is  easy  to  calculate  the 
ratio  of  the  meantone  fifth:  the  major  third  has  the  ratio  5:4; 
hence  the  ratio  of  the  tone  will  be  the  square  root  of  this,  or 
/J5:2.  The  ratio  of  the  major  ninth  will  be  twice  the  ratio  of  the 
tone,  or  ^5:1.  The  ratio  of  the  fifth  will  be  the  square  root  of 
the  ratio  of  the  ninth,  or  4V57l.  If  we  consider  the  syntonic  comma 
to  be  21.5  cents,  a  fifth  diminished  by  1/4  comma  will  be  702.0- 
5.4  =  696.6  cents. 

The  deviation  for  the  meantone  temperament  is  nearly  as 
large  as  for  just  intonation.  That  would  seem  to  indicate  that 
temperament  makes  for  little  improvement.  Strangely  enough, 
this  is  absolutely  true,  so  far  as  the  remote  keys  are  concerned. 
However,  if  the  deviation  were  to  be  measured  only  from  E^  to 
G*,  without  allowing  for  the  enharmonic  uses  of  notes,  the  mean- 
tone  temperament  would  be  an  easy  victor  over  just  intonation. 
That  is,  if  we  were  computing  the  deviation  of  eleven  fifths  only, 
omitting  the  wolf  fifth  of  737  cents,  the  standard  deviation  for  the 
meantone  temperament  would  be  much  smaller  than  that  for  just 
intonation.  But,  since  our  ideal  is  equal  temperament,  the  de- 
viation as  computed  shows  accurately  enough  how  very  unsatis- 
factory this  tuning  is  when  its  narrow  bounds  are  overstepped. 

The  meantone  temperament  was  used  from  the  beginning  upon 
keyboard  instruments  only.  It  was  the  temperament  that  Vicen- 
tino  intended  for  his  Archicembalo  when  he  said  that  it  may  be 
tuned  "justly  with  the  temperament  of  the  flattened  fifth,  accord- 
ing to  the  usage  and  tuning  common  to  all  the  keyboard  instru- 
ments, as  organs,  cembali,  clavichords,  and  the  like."**  Zarlino 
called  the  meantone  temperament  a  "new  temperament"  and  said 
that  it  is  "very  pleasing  for  all  purposes"  when  used  on  key- 
board instruments. 9  To  divide  the  major  third  into  two  mean 
tones,  Zarlino  advocated  the  Euclidean  construction  for  a  mean 


8See  Chapter  VI. 

^Gioseffo  Zarlino,  Dimostrationi  armoniche  (Venice,  1571),  p.  267. 

27 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


proportional,  and  of  course  the  fifth  could  be  constructed  from 
the  major  ninth  by  the  same  means. 

Verheijen's  reply  to  Stevin's  discussion  of  equal  temperament 
explained  the  meantone  temperament  in  detail  10  He  even  in- 
cluded amonochord  for  it  (Table  23),  and  thus  has  the  distinction 
of  being  the  first  person,  so  far  as  we  know,  to  put  its  ratios  into 
figures  (cents  values  as  in  Aron,  Table  22,  beginning  with  F  as 
503). 

Table  23.  Verheijen's  Monochord  for  Meantone  Temperament 
Lengths  10000  9750  8944  8560  8000  7477  7155  6687  6400  5961 

7  19  13  1  3 

Names         F°        F#"    G"      G#"    A*"1      Bb+4     b"*~      C~*     C#"2    D" 
5590     5350    5000 

1  5 

Eb+-    E"         Fo 

In  Spain,  Sancta  Maria  described  a  practical  tuning  system 
that  may  have  been  the  same  as  the  meantone  tuning. H  He  said 
that  on  the  clavichord  and  thevihuela  (the  Spanish  lute)  each  fifth 
is  to  be  "a  little  flat."  In  fact,  the  diminution  is  to  be  "so  small 
that  it  can  scarcely  be  noticed."  Since  he  did  not  say  whether  the 
thirds  were  to  be  pure  or  a  little  sharp,  we  cannot  know  whether 
his  system  was  the  real  meantone  or  came  nearer  equal  tem- 
perament. However,  he  held  that  a  tone  cannot  be  divided  into 
two  equal  semitones,  and  consistently  made  the  diatonic  semi- 
tone larger  than  the  chromatic  semitone,  as  it  would  be  in  just 
intonation  or  the  meantone  temperament. 

The  first  German  writer  to  describe  the  meantone  tempera- 
ment was  more  explicit.    This  was  Michael  Praetor ius,* 2  m  a 

lOsimon  Stevin,  Van  de  Spiegeling  der  Singconst,  ed.  D.  Bierens  de  Haan 
(Amsterdam,  1884).  Verheijen's  letter  is  in  Appendice  A.  Both  discussion 
and  reply  remained  in  manuscript  for  almost  three  hundred  years. 

^Tom^s  de  Sancta  Maria,  Arte  de  taner  fantasia  (Valladolid,  1565),  Chapter 
53. 

12syntagma  musicum  (Wolfenbuttel,  1618),  Vol.  II;  new  edition,  1884-94,  pub- 
lished as  Publikation  alterer  praktischen  und  theoretischen  Musikwerke, 
Band  13,  pp.  178 ff. 

28 


MEANTONE  TEMPERAMENT 


chapter  on  the  tuning  of  the  "Regal,  Clavicymbel,  Symphonien  und 
dergleichen  Instrument."  His  was  a  practical  system,  with  major 
thirds  and  octaves  pure,  and  fifths  flat.  Praetor ius  explained 
carefully  how  various  intervals  are  altered  by  fractional  parts 
of  the  comma. 

Otto  Gibelius^  showed  a  method  for  obtaining  an  approxi- 
mately correct  monochord  for  the  meantone  temperament.  First 
he  made  a  table  in  which  were  shown  pairs  of  numbers  differing 
by  the  syntonic  comma  for  every  note  in  a  14-note  octave,  ex- 
tending from  A*3  toD#.  Then  he  made  an  arithmetical  division 
of  each  comma,  with  3/4,  1/2,  or  1/4  comma  subtracted  from 
the  larger  number,  to  obtain  the  tempered  value.  C,  E,  G^,  and 
A*3  needed  no  temperament  (see  Table  24).  His  results  check 
closely  with  numbers  obtained  by  taking  roots. ^  For  example, 
his  D  is  193200;  it  should  be  193196.  His  G  is  144450  instead  of 
144447.  Since  the  comma  is  small  relative  to  the  intervals  of 
the  scale  and  since  as  much  as  a  quarter  or  a  half  of  it  is  used, 
the  error  could  not  be  great.  An  arithmetical  division  of  the 
ditonic  comma  into  twelfths  in  the  construction  of  equal  temper- 
ament would  create  greater  errors  than  this  for  certain  notes  of 
the  division. 

Table  24.    Gibelius'  Monochord  for  Meantone  Temperament 

Lengths  216000    206720    193200    184896    180562.5    172800    161500    154560 
Names         C°         C#"       D"         D*"*       Eb+*  E_1  F+4  F#" 

144450    138240    135000    129200    120750    115560    108000 
G"5         G#"2       Ab+1         A"        Bb+2        B"  C° 

Lemme  Rossi,*  ^  writing  in  the  same  year  as  Gibelius,  would 
have  approved  the  latter 's  approximation  for  the  meantone  tem- 

1,jPropositiones  mathematico-musicae  (Mlinden,  1666),  copperplate  opposite 
page  14 

l^Wolffgang  Caspar  Printz,  Phrynis  Mytilenaeus  oder  der  satyrische  Com- 
ponist  (Dresden  and  Leipzig,  1696),  p.  73. 

l^sistema  musico  (Perugia,  1666),  p.  59. 

29 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


perament,  for  he  himself  said  that  the  arithmetical  division  of 
the  comma  differs  "insensibly"  from  a  geometrical  division.  In 
the  example  that  he  gave,  the  geometrical  mean  between  the  two 
numbers,  31104  and  30720,  in  the  ratio  of  81  to  80,  is  30911,  and 
the  arithmetical  mean  is  30912,  certainly  a  negligible  difference. 
But,  he  said,  the  correct  string- lengths  for  the  meantone  tem- 
perament can  be  obtained  both  "easily  and  quickly  with  the  table 
of  logarithms." 

Our  final  monochord  for  the  meantone  temperament  proper 
will  be  Rossi's  "Numeri  del  sistema  participate "16  He  has 
given  it  for  a  19-note  octave  commencing  on  A  (see  Table  25). 
Since  C  itself  is  a  tempered  value  here,  we  have  transposed  the 
system  up  a  minor  third  from  A  to  C,  selecting  those  notes  that 
would  belong  to  the  ordinary  meantone  scale.  The  number  used 
for  his  fundamental  had  been  previously  used  in  a  table  of  just 
intonation. 


Table  25.  Rossi's  Monochord  for  Meantone  Temperament 

Lengths 
Names 

41472 
C° 

24806 

3 

A" 

39690 

7 

23184 

37095 

i 

d" 

22187 

5 

B" 

34668 
Eb+^ 

20736 
C° 

33178 

-i 
E 

31008 
F+i 

29676 

3 

F#~2 

27734 
i 

G"  . 

26542 

G#"2 

Another  sort  of  approximation  connected  with  the  meantone 
temperament  was  given  by  Claas  Douwes.l^  In  describing  the 
bonded  clavichord  he  gave  simple  ratios  (most  of  them  super- 
particular)  for  various  intervals  that  would  occur  on  the  same 
string.  For  example,  the  highest  string  has  C,  B,  B*3,  and  A. 
C-A  is  6:5;  B-A,  19:17;  Bb-A,  15:14.  On  the  next  string,  G#-F 
is  7:6.  Two  octaves  lower,  the  ninth  string  has  only  two  notes, 
G#  and  G,  with  the  ratio  24:23. 

Dou\  ^s  had  explained  that  his  was  a  tempered  system.  His 
rational  ratios  are  good  approximations  to  the  surds  of  the  mean- 

16lbid.,  p.  83. 

l^Grondig  Ondersoek  van  de  Toonen  der  Musijk  (Franeker,  1699),  pp.  98-104. 

30 


MEANTONE  TEMPERAMENT 


tone  temperament.  His  minor  third,  with  ratio  6:5,  is  316  cents; 
the  meantone  minor  third  is  310.  His  augmented  second,  7:6,  is 
267  cents;  the  meantone  augmented  second  is  270.  His  tone, 
19:17,  is  almost  193  cents;  the  meantone  tone  is  practically  the 
same.  His  diatonic  semitone,  15:14,  is  119  cents;  the  meantone 
diatonic  semitone,  117.  His  chromatic  semitone,  24:23,  is  74 
cents;  the  meantone  chromatic  semitone,  76.  His  system  agrees 
with  itself  as  well  as  with  the  ordinary  meantone  system.  For 
example,  the  tone  should  be  the  sum  of  the  diatonic  and  the  chro- 
matic semitones,  or  15/14  x  24/23.  This  product  is  3420:3059; 
his  ratio  for  the  tone,  19:17,  equals  3420:3060,  a  close  corre- 
spondence. 

In  tracing  the  later  history  of  the  meantone  temperament,  it 
would  be  easy  to  name  theorists  in  all  the  principal  European 
countries  who  continued  to  favor  an  unequal  tuning  of  keyboard 
instruments  later  than  the  first  quarter  of  the  eighteenth  century. 
But,  unless,  like  Galin  in  1818,  they  specifically  say  that  they 
favor  the  tuning  in  which  the  fifths  are  tempered  by  1/4  syntonic 
comma  or  its  equivalent  (31-division)/^  we  have  no  right  to  call 
their  methods  the  meantone  temperament.  This  is  the  fallacy  of 
so  much  that  has  been  written  on  this  subject. 

Other  Varieties  of  Meantone  Temperament 

Strictly,  there  is  only  one  meantone  temperament.  But  theo- 
rists have  been  inclined  to  lump  together  under  that  head  all 
sorts  of  systems  intended  for  keyboard  instruments.  For  ex- 
ample, the  statement  often  appears  in  print  that  in  England  the 
meantone  temperament  was  used  for  organs  until  the  middle  of 
the  nineteenth  century.  William  Crotch, 1*  writing  early  inthat 
century,  wrote:  "As  organs  are  at  present  tuned,  (with  unequal 
temperament),  keys  which  have  many  flats  or  sharps  will  not 
have  a  good  effect,  especially  if  the  time  be  slow."  That  state- 
ment is  enough  to  cause  a  host  of  later  English  writers  to  say 

18pierre  Galin,  Exposition  d'une  nouvelle  me'thode  pour  l'enseignement  de  la 
musique  (3rd  edition,  Bordeaux  and  Paris,  1862;   1st  edition,  1818). 

^Elements  of  Musical  Composition  (London,  1812),  p.  112. 

31 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


that  Crotch  reported  the  meantone  temperament  to  be  in  use  in 
his  age. 

But  later  in  his  book  Crotch  had  this  to  say:  "Unequal  tem- 
perament is  that  wherein  some  of  the  fifths,  and  consequently 
some  of  the  thirds,  are  made  more  perfect  than  on  the  equal 
temperament,  which  necessarily  renders  others  less  perfect. 
Of  this  there  are  many  systems,  which  the  student  is  now  capable 
of  examining  for  himself. "20  jn  other  words,  Crotch  is  saying 
that  there  was  a  great  diversity  in  the  tuning  of  organs  in  his  day. 

In  Chapter  VII,  "Irregular  Systems,"  twenty-odd  men  are 
mentioned  who  collectively  have  described  fifty  of  the  "many 
systems,"  none  of  which  is  the  meantone  temperament.  In  the 
present  chapter  we  propose  to  describe  still  other  systems  of 
temperament,  systems  formed  on  the  same  general  pattern  as 
meantone  temperament.  Bosanquet  called  "regular"  a  tempera- 
ment constructed  with  one  size  of  fifth. **■  The  Pythagorean  tun- 
ing, equal  temperament,  meantone  temperament— all  are  regular 
systems.  The  systems  that  follow  are  also  regular,  with  values 
for  the  fifth  smaller  than  that  of  equal  temperament  and  (usually) 
larger  than  that  of  the  meantone  temperament.  Since  their  con- 
struction is  similar,  it  is  easy  to  describe  them  as  varieties  of 
the  meantone  temperament.  In  all  of  them,  the  tone  is  precisely 
half  of  the  major  third.  No  harm  will  be  done  by  such  a  nomen- 
clature if  we  realize  that  these  are  regular  temperaments  which 
the  earlier  theorists  themselves  considered  of  the  same  type  as 
the  1/4 -comma  temperament  and  some  of  which  they  preferred 
to  it. 

The  first  regular  temperament  to  be  advocated  after  the  de- 
scription of  the  ordinary  meantone  temperament  was  that  de- 
scribed by  Zarlino  in  which  "each  fifth  remains  diminished  and 
imperfect  by  2/7  comma. "22  Although  Zarlino  showed  a  mono- 
chord  with  this  tuning  for  the  diatonic  genus  only,  he  intended  it 

20lbid.,  p.  135. 

21R.H  M.  Bosanquet,  An  Elementary  Treatise  on  Musical  Intervals  and  Tem- 
perament  (London,  1876),  Chapter  VTJI. 

22cioseffo  Zarlino,  Istitutioni  armoniche  (Venice,  1558),  pp.  126 ff. 

32 


MEANTONE  TEMPERAMENT 


also  for  the  chromatic  genus— by  which  he  meant  the  ordinary 
black  keys.  He  also  described  an  enharmonic  genus,  having  19 
notes  to  the  octave,  as  applied  to  a  cembalo  which  Master  Dom- 
enico  Pesarese  had  made  for  him.  This  must  have  had  the  same 
tuning,  although  Zarlino  did  not  clearly  say  so.  Most  of  these 
varieties  of  the  meantone  temperament  will  have  a  smaller  de- 
viation when  applied  to  a  keyboard  with  19  or  more  notes  to  the 
octave  than  upon  the  usual  keyboard.  Zarlino's  temperament 
corresponds  to  the  50-division,  and,  as  such,  will  be  discussed 
in  the  chapter  on  multiple  division. 

In  Table  26,  we  see  the  2/7-comma  temperament  applied  to 
a  keyboard  with  12  notes  to  the  octave.  Since  the  amount  of  tem- 
pering is  greater  than  1/4  comma,  the  deviation  is  greater  than 
for  Aron's  system.  It  is,  in  fact,  a  very  poor  system,  and  Zar- 
lino later  admitted  it  to  be  inferior  to  the  1/4- comma  system. 
The  only  just  interval  in  it  is  the  chromatic  semitone.  Tanaka 
liked  it  "because  all  the  imperfect  consonances  are  impure 
alike, "23  that  is,  the  major  and  minor  thirds  are  1/7  comma  flat 
(3  cents),  and  the  major  and  minor  sixths  are  sharp  by  the  same 
amount.  To  construct  it  on  a  monochord,  Zarlino  would  use  the 
questionable  virtues  of  the  mesolabium.24 

Table  26.    Zarlino's  2/7  -  Comma  Temperament 

4  6  82  _12  2  _16  6  +4  10 

Names     C°  C#       D"    Eb   7    E~7    F   7    F#"  '    G~7   G#"  7    A~7    Bb  *    B-  7    C° 
Cents       0     70        191     313      383     504    574      696     817       887     1008    1078  1200 
M.D.   25.0;     S.D.  25.3 

The  next  variety  of  meantone  temperament  is  also  highly  un- 
satisfactory when  applied  to  an  octave  of  twelve  semitones.  This 
is  the  1/3-comma  temperament,  the  invention  of  Francisco  Sali- 
nas, which  he  described  as  follows:  "The  first  of  them  [the  other 
two  were  the  2/7-comma  and  the  1/4-comma  temperaments]  has 
the  comma  divided  into  three  parts  equally  proportional,  of  which 


23Shoh^  Tanaka,   "Studien  im  Gebiete  der  reinen  Stimmung,"  Vierteljahrs- 
schrift  fur  Musikwissenschaft,  VI  (1890),  65. 


24  For  an  account  of  the  mesolabium,  see  the  second  part  of  Chapter  IV. 

33 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


the  minor  tone  is  increased  by  one  part  and  the  major  tone  is 
decreased  by  two  parts. "25  Salinas  showed  that  his  method  re- 
sults in  pure  minor  thirds,  tritone,  and  major  sixth.  But  the 
fifth  is  diminished  by  1/3  comma,  and  so  is  the  major  third.  On 
the  whole  this  tuning  does  not  compare  favorably  with  the  others, 
but  Salinas  added:  "Although  this  imperfection  is  seen  to  be 
greater  than  that  which  is  found  in  the  other  two  temperaments, 
nevertheless  it  is  endurable." 

Salinas  intended  his  temperament  for  an  octave  containing  19 
notes,  divided  into  the  three  genera— diatonic,  chromatic,  and 
enharmonic.  His  special  reason  for  advocating  this  tuning  was 
the  ease  of  realizing  it  upon  the  monochord.  Seven  of  the  notes 
can  be  obtained  by  a  series  of  just  minor  thirds  below  and  above 
the  fundamental.  Thus  we  obtain  C,  D#,  E°,  F#,  Gb,  A,  and  B#, 
and  Salinas  has  given  their  string- lengths  for  the  octave  22500 
to  11250. 

To  find  the  notes  D  and  E,  two  mean  proportionals  must  be 
inserted  in  the  tritone,  C— F^.  This  "will  be  very  easy  to  those 
who  know  the  use  of  a  certain  instrument  invented  by  Archimedes, 
which  is  called  mesolabium,  from  finding  mean  lines  by  it."  The 
remainder  of  the  notes  can  then  be  obtained  by  minor  thirds 
from  D  and  E. 

We  agree  with  Salinas  that  the  thirds  and  especially  the  fifths 
of  the  1/3-comma  temperament  are  less  pleasing  than  those  of 
the  other  two.  But,  in  addition  to  its  being  capable  of  quicker 
tuning  than  the  Zarlinian  2/7-comma  method,  it  has  an  advantage 
possessed  by  neither  of  the  other  methods:  it  is  practically  a 
closed  or  cyclic  system.  Among  its  19  notes  there  is  no  fifth 
containing  a  wolf;  nor  are  there  any  discordant  thirds.  It  is  an 
equal  temperament  of  19  notes. 

In  recent  times  the  19-division  has  had  eloquent  advocates, 
to  whom  reference  is  made  in  the  chapter  on  multiple  division. 
Let  us  see  how  well  the  1/3-comma  system  is  adapted  to  a  12- 
note  keyboard.  As  Table  27  shows,  this  is  the  poorest  tuning  of 
all— like  Zarlino's  method,  it  is  worse  than  just  intonation.  How- 
ever, too  many  theorists  who  have  described  these  two  systems 
have  neglected  to  add  that  they  are  excellent  foral9-note  octave. 


2&De  musica  libri  VII,  p.  143. 

34 


MEANTONE  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  27.    Salinas'  1 /3  -  Comma  Temperament 

7  2  +1  44-L  2  l  8  +2  5 

Names     C°  C^"  D~3   Eb      E"   F   s   f#~    G"  G#"  a"     Bb  3  B"  C° 
Cents         0      64      190    316     379     505    569     695    758    884  1010    1074  1200 


M.D.   30.3;     S.D.  30.7 


It  would  help  us  in  portraying  an  orderly  development  of  the 
12-note  temperaments  if  we  could  show  that  little  by  little  the 
temperament  of  the  fifth  was  reduced  from  the  1/4  comma  of  the 
meantone  temperament  to  the  1/11  comma  (1/12  ditonic  comma 
equals  1/11  syntonic  comma)  of  equal  temperament.  Probably 
there  was  such  a  tendency.  But  it  is  only  a  fortunate  accident 
that  Verheijen  included  the  ratio  of  the  fifth  for  the  1/5-comma 
temperament,  together  with  the  ratios  for  the  three  temperaments 
discussed  by  Zarlino  and  Salinas. 26  Verheijen's  first  ratio  for 
the  fifth  is  the  cube  root  of  10:3  (1/3-comma  temperament);  then 
the  fourth  root  of  5:1  (1/4-comma);  the  fifth  root  of  15:2  (1/5- 
comma);  the  seventh  root  of  50:3  (2/7-comma).  Verheijen's 
casual  reference  to  the  1/5-comma  temperament  indicates  that 
even  then  some  people  were  using  it.  Rossi,  a  couple  of  genera- 
tions later,  also  referred  briefly  to  the  1/5-comma  temperament, 
including  it  as  one  of  the  regular  types  then  in  use. ^ 

The  temperament  shown  in  Table  28  has  in  its  favor,  like  the 
1/3-comma  temperament,  the  equal  distortion  of  the  fifths  and 
the  major  thirds,  the  former  being  1/5  comma  flat,  the  latter 
sharp  by  the  same  amount.    In  it  the  diatonic  semitone  is  pure. 


Table  28.    1/5  -  Comma  Temperament  (Verheijen,  Rossi) 

7  2  3  4  1  6  1  8  3,2 

Names     C°  C#'z  D"5  Eb  '  e"   F    ■  P*"  G"  G#"  A"  Bb   »  B_1  C° 
Cents        0      83        195    307     390     502    586     698     781     893    1005   1088  1200 
M.D.    14.0;     S.D.    14.2 

''"Simon  Stevin,  Van  de  Spiegeling  der  Singconst,  Appendice  D. 
2'Sistema  musico,  p.  58. 

35 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


The  deviation  of  this  temperament  is  only  about  two-thirds  that 
of  the  1/4-comma  system. 

There  is  an  odd  reference  to  the  1/5- comma  temperament. 
Dechales^S  gave  a  monochord  which  he  called  the  "Diatonic 
scale  of  Guido  of  Arezzo."  It  is,  however,  a  chromatic  scale, 
and,  so  far  as  can  be  ascertained,  has  nothing  in  common  with 
any  of  the  ideas  expressed  by  Guido. 

It  seems  evident  that  Dechaleshas  intended  the  monochord  in 
Table  29  for  the  1/5-comma  temperament.  Its  ninth  note  differs 
greatly  from  the  cents  value  given  in  the  previous  table;  but  the 
note  is  A^  in  Dechales'  monochord  and  would  naturally  be  more 
than  a  comma  higher  than  the  G^  more  commonly  used.  Other 
divergences  can  be  explained  by  the  fact  that  Dechales  has  not 
expressed  his  numbers  with  great  accuracy.  However,  the  mean 
value  for  his  diatonic  semitone  is  111.4,  against  112.0  for  the 
1/5-comma  temperament;  for  his  chromatic  semitone,  84.0  cents 
against  83.2.  How  he  reached  the  conclusion  that  Guido  favored 
such  a  temperament  remains  a  mystery.  Actually  Dechales  him- 
self ascribed  the  1/4-comma  temperament  to  Guido  (rather  than 
the  1/5-comma),  contrary  to  the  evidence  of  this  monochord. 

Table  29.    Dechales'  "Guidonian"  Temperament  (1/5  -  Comma) 

Lengths  60    57i    53|    50^    47|    44^  42|    40|    37|    35^    33i    31§  30 

Names      C     C#     D       Eb       E        F  F#     G       Ab       A       Bb       B       C 

Cents        0     85     194     312     395     502  587     696  808    893    1009  1090  1200 

M.D.   13.3;  S.D.   13.8 

The  1/5-comma  variety  of  meantone  temperament  comes 
close  to  the  43-division.  As  such,  it  is  discussed  briefly  in 
Chapter  VI,  with  the  principal  reference  to  Sauveur. 

Another  temperament  discussed  by  Rossi^  has  its  fifths 
flattened  by  2/9  comma  (see  Table  30).  He  merely  called  it 
"another  tempered  system,"  without  ascribing  it  to  any  theorist. 
Romieu  identified  this  temperament  with  the  31 -division,  and  thus 


28cursus  seu  mundus  mathematicus,  p.  20. 

29sistema  musico,  p.  64. 

36 


MEANTONE  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  30.    Rossi's  2/9  -  Comma  Temperament 

14  4  282  42  16  2  4  _10 

Names     C°  C#"~  D"  E°+S  e"   F+a  F#"  G"  G#~  9    A"  Bb+9  B_  9    C° 
Cents         0     79        194     308      389     503    582    697    777      892    1006     1085  1200 
M.D.   17.0;     S.D.   17.2 

credited  it  to  Huyghens.^O  Actually,  as  we  have  already  said, 
the  1/4-comma  temperament  comes  closest  to  the  31-division. 
But  perhaps  other  writers  before  Romieu  confused  these  tem- 
peraments. For  example,  Printz^l  spoke  of  a  "still  earlier" 
temperament  that  takes  2/9  comma  from  each  fifth— earlier, 
perhaps,  than  Zarlino's  2/7- comma  temperament,  which  he  had 
been  previously  discussing.  He  also  might  have  meant  Vicentino's 
31-division,  since  there  are  no  early  references  to  the  2/9- 
comma  temperament. 

Since  2/9  is  the  harmonic  mean  between  1/4  and  1/5,  the  de- 
viation for  this  temperament  is  approximately  the  mean  of  the 
deviations  of  the  other  two  temperaments.  Like  Zarlino's  2/7- 
eomma  temperament,  its  third  is  altered  half  as  much  as  its 
fifth,  being  1/9  comma  sharp.  Its  augmented  second,  as  F-G#, 
is  pure.  The  74-division  corresponds  to  the  2/9-comma  tem- 
perament, and  Drobisch  liked  this  division  best  of  all  systems 
that  form  their  major  thirds  regularly. 

Schneegass  gave  an  interesting  geometrical  construction  for 
what  was  much  like  the  common  meantone  temperament,  but 
more  like  the  2/9-comma  temperament.  His  contention  was 
that  the  diatonic  semitone  contains  3  1/4  "commas"  and  the 
chromatic  semitone  2  1/4.  (These  commas  of  35.3  cents  have 
nothing  in  common  with  either  the  ditonic  [23. 5J  or  the  syntonic 
[2I.5]  comma).  Thus  the  tone  contains  5  1/2  commas,  and  the 
octave  5x5  1/2  +  2x3  1/4  =  34  commas.  As  is  shown  in  Chapter 
VI,  the  34 -division  has  fifths  that  are  almost  4  cents  too  large 

30jean-Baptiste  Romieu,  "Memoire  theorique  &  pratique  sur  les  systemes 
tempe'res  de  musique,"  Me'moires  de  Tacademie  royale  des  sciences,  1758, 
p.  837. 

31Phrynis  Mytilenaeus  oder  der  satyrische  Componist,  p.  88. 

32Cyriac  Schneegass,  Nova  &  exquisita  monochordi  dimensio  (Erfurt,  1590), 
Chapter  HI. 

37 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


and  thirds  that  are  2  cents  too  large.  But  this  was  not  what 
Schneegass  had  in  mind.  His  theoretical  fifth  had  the  ratio 
160:107,  or  696.6  cents,  which  is  precisely  the  size  of  the  mean- 
tone  fifth,  and  he  directed  that  this  ratio  be  used  twice  to  form 
the  tone. 

Then  came  the  application  of  the  doctrine  about  commas:  A 
right  triangle  was  to  be  constructed,  with  the  space  of  the  tone, 
G-A,  as  base,  and  thrice  this  length  for  the  altitude  (see  Fig- 
ure A).  Note  that  "space"  here  does  not  refer  to  the  total  length 
of  a  line,  but  rather  to  the  distance  from  one  point  of  division  to 
another  Since  3  1/4:2  1/4  =  13:9, the  acute  angle  at  thetopwas 
to  be  divided  in  the  ratio  of  13:9,  with  the  larger  angle  toward 
A.  The  point  where  this  line  cut  the  base  was  to  be  G#.  Now 
tan"1 1/3  =  18°  26',  and  13/22  of  this  angle  is  10°  53'.  The  space 
between  G#  and  A,  then,  would  be  3 tan  10°  53'  =  .57681  of  the 
space  between  G  and  A.  From  the  figures  in  his  table,  the  divi- 
sion was  made  with  extreme  care.  The  ratio  in  the  table  of  the 
space  from  G*  to  A  to  the  space  from  G  to  A  is  15/26  or  .57692. 
By  a  series  of  lines  parallel  to  the  base,  he  cleverly  divided  the 
other  tones  (Bb-C,  C-D,  Eb-F,  and  F-G)  into  chromatic  and 
diatonic  semitones  proportional  to  the  division  of  G-A. 


Fig.  A.    Schneegass'  Division  of  the  Monochord 

Reproduced  by  courtesy  of  the  Sibley  Library*  of  the 
Eastman  School  of  Music 


38 


MEANTONE  TEMPERAMENT 


To  examine  the  assumption  that  Schneegass  made,  let  us  des- 
ignate as  a  the  angle  10°  53'  and  as  ft  the  angle  18°  26 ',  and  as 
L  the  length  for  the  note  A.  Then  the  length  for  G  was  L  +  tan  a  , 
and  for  G  it  was  L  +  tan  ft  .    His  assumption: 

log/L  +  tanff  ):  log/ L  +  tan  a    \=  ft  :  a 


In  general  this  would  be  only  a  rough  approximation.  In  this  case, 
where  ft  :  a  =  22:13,  it  works  very  well  indeed. 

Schneegass' actual  fifth,  G-D,of  698.1  cents  is  a  little  larger 
than  his  theoretical  fifth  of  696.6,  and  the  mean  of  all  11  good 
fifths  is  697.2  cents.  This  last  figure  is  precisely  the  fifth  of 
the  2/9 -comma  temperament.  The  mean  value  of  his  tones  is 
194.0  cents,  as  compared  with  194.4  cents  of  the  2/9- comma 
temperament,  and  his  geometrical  division  of  the  tones  yields 
semitones  of  113.9  and  80.1  cents,  compared  with  114.0  and  80.4 
cents. 

Schneegass'  actual  fifth  has  approximately  the  ratio  226:151, 
instead  of  his  theoretical  160:107.  It  is  idle  to  speculate  why  his 
figures  fail  to  correspond  with  his  theory,  or  why  they  agree  so 
beautifully  with  the  2/9 -comma  temperament.  The  significant 
thing  is  that  they  agree  so  well  with  themselves,  which  is  an  in- 
dication of  the  soundness  of  his  mathematics!  There  is,  how- 
ever, one  puzzling  clue  to  his  division  of  the  tone.  Suppose  the 
space  of  the  tone  G-A  had  been  divided  arithmetically  in  the  ratio 
of  13:9,  instead  of  the  more  complicated  division  of  the  angle 
actually  used.  Then  Schneegass'  G*  would  have  been  at  86.100 
instead  of  at  85.967.  This  would  have  made  the  G*  3.3  cents 
lower  than  in  the  table,  and  his  tone  would  have  been  divided  into 
semitones  of  117.7  and  76.0  cents.  Nowthe  semitones  of  the  1/4- 
comma  temperament  are  of  117.1  and  76.0  cents  respectively. 
Thus  an  arithmetical  division  of  his  tones  would  have  come  close 
to  the  temperament  which  is  suggested  by  his  theoretical  fifth. 
However,  his  actual  division  (Table  31)  with  a  15:11  ratio,  is 
very  consistent  with  itself,  as  well  as  with  the  2/9 -comma  tem- 
perament. 


39 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  31.    Schneegass'  Variety  of  Meantone  Temperament 

Lengths  90.000    85.967    80.467    75.267    71.867    67.267    64.200    60.133 
Names         G  G#  A  Bb  B  C  C#  D 

Cents  0  79  194         309         389         504         585         698 

56.300    53.750    50.367    48.083    45.000 
Eb  E  F  F*  G 

812         892  1005       1085       1200 

M.D.   16.7;     S.D.   16.9 

Robert  Smith"^  is  responsible  for  three  wholly  unsatisfactory 
varieties  of  the  meantone  temperament.  He  told  first  of  a  Mr. 
Harrison,  who  tuned  his  viol  by  "taking  the  interval  of  the  major 
third  to  that  of  the  octave,  as  the  diameter  of  a  circle  to  its  cir- 
cumference—  It  follows  from  Mr.  Harrison's  assumption,  that 
his  3rd  major  is  tempered  flat  by  a  full  fifth  of  a  comma."  If  the 
ratio  of  the  major  third  to  the  octave  is  l:tr  ,  the  third  will  have 
382.0  cents,  or  be  1/5  comma  flat,  as  Smith  said.  The  fifth  will 
then  be  tempered  by  3/10  comma.  Romieu^  barely  mentioned 
3/10-  and  3/11 -comma  temperaments,  but  did  not  discuss  them 
on  the  ground  that  they  were  too  like  temperaments  with  unity  in 
the  numerator.  Except  for  a  few  references  to  Smith  and  this 
tuning  by  rr,  the  3/10- comma  temperament  has  escaped  further 
notice  (see  Table  32). 

Table  32.    Harrison's  3/10  -  Comma  Temperament 

21_  3  _9_  63  2  3_  12  9  3  3 

Names     C°  C#~10  D"  Eb+l°  E"  F  w  F*"  G"10  G#~~  A~a  BD   *  B"  C° 
Cents       0     69         191    314      382    504     573     696     764      887     1009  1078  1200 
M.D.   26.2;     S.D.   26.6 

Since  3/10  is  about  the  same  as  2/7,  the  deviation  for  this 
temperament  is  approximately  the  same  as  for  Zarlino's,  both 

■^Harmonics,  or  the  Philosophy  of  Musical  Sounds  (Cambridge,  1749),  pp.  xi, 
xii. 

3^In  Memoires  de  l'acade'mie  royale  des  sciences,  1758,  p.  827. 
40 


MEANTONE  TEMPERAMENT 


being  inferior  to  just  intonation.  It  has  no  special  features  to 
recommend  it,  since  its  one  natural  feature,  the  Tr  ratio,  is 
something  to  be  determined  by  ear  or  by  logarithms,  and  would 
not  make  the  construction  of  a  monochord  any  simpler. 

After  referring  to  Harrison's  system,  as  quoted  above,  Smith 
continued,  "My  third  determined  by  theory,  upon  the  principle  of 
making  all  the  concords  within  the  extent  of  every  three  octaves 
as  equally  harmonious  as  possible,  is  tempered  flat  by  one  ninth 
of  a  comma;  or  almost  one  eighth,  when  no  more  concords  are 
taken  into  the  calculation  than  what  are  contained  within  one  oc- 
tave." Later  he  showed  that  "to  have  all  the  concords  in  four 
octaves  made  equally  harmonious,"  the  thirds  will  be  1/10  comma 
flat.  35 

With  the  third  flat  by  1/9  comma,  the  fifth  will  be  tempered 
by  5/18  comma,  a  quantity  impossible  to  judge  by  ear.  In  the 
second  temperament,  with  the  third  1/10  comma  flat,  the  fifth 
will  be  11/40  comma  flat.  The  difference  between  these  values 
of  the  fifth  is  only  1/360  comma!  Therefore  the  temperaments 
would  not  vary  for  any  note  by  as  much  as  one  cent.  For  this 
reason  only  the  first  of  Smith's  temperaments  is  shown  in  Table 
33. 

Table  33.    Smith's  5/18  -  Comma  Temperament 

35  5  5  _10  5  5  5  20  5  +5  25 

Names  C°  C#~18  D"  Eb  *  E"  9  F  '»  F*"  G"15  G#"  9  A"  Bb  s  B"18  C° 
Cents    0  72    192  312  384   504  576  696  768   888  1008  1080  1200 
M.D.  23.3;  S.D.  23.7 

Since  5/18  is  also  approximately  the  same  as  2/7,  Smith's 
temperament  is  only  a  little  better  than  Zarlino's.  We  have  pre- 
viously indicated  that  the  50-division  has  usually  been  considered 
the  equivalent  of  the  2/7-comma  temperament.  Smith  asserts, 
however,  that  his  temperament  corresponds  to  the  50-division, 
the  error  of  the  fifth  in  the  latter  being  41/148  comma.  He  is 
entirely  correct  in  his  claim. 

Smith  did  not  suggest,  however,  that  the  octave  be  divided  into 
fifty  parts— merely  that  "a  system  of  rational  intervals  deduced 

35smith,  Harmonics,  p.  171. 

41 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


from  dividing  the  octave  into  50  equal  parts, ...will  differ  insensi- 
bly from  the  system  of  equal  harmony."  His  desire  is  more 
modest— to  have  at  least  21  different  pitches  in  the  octave,  pro- 
perly to  differentiate  the  sharps,  naturals,  and  flats.  On  the  or- 
gan and  harpsichord  this  could  be  done  by  adding  extra  pipes  and 
strings.  Performance  would  be  facilitated  by  having  "seven 
couples  of  secondary  notes,"  governed  by  stops,  so  that  the  ap- 
propriate notes  for  a  particular  piece  could  be  chosen.  Of  course, 
upon  an  instrument  with  19  notes  to  the  octave  (the  other  two 
would  be  of  little  use),  Smith's  temperament,  like  Zarlino's  and 
Salinas',  would  be  far  more  acceptable  than  on  the  ordinary  key- 
board. Smith  himself  considered  that  ordinary  equal  tempera- 
ment "far  exceeds"  both  the  31- and  50-divisions,  because  of  the 
cumbersomeness  of  the  latter  systems. 

The  only  other  important  variety  of  the  meantone  temperament 
was  that  practiced  by  Silbermann  and  his  contemporaries.  Ac- 
cording to  Sorge,  Silbermann  tempered  his  fifths  by  1/6  comma.36 
Since  Sorge  himself  made  no  distinction  between  the  syntonic 
and  ditonic  commas,  we  might  divide  either.  If  we  divide  the 
ditonic  comma,  the  deviation  is  precisely  the  same  as  for  the 
Pythagorean  tuning,  M.D.  11.7,  S.D.  11.8.  But,  for  better  com- 
parison with  the  other  varieties  of  meantone  temperament,  let 
us  divide  the  syntonic  comma.  Then  the  major  third  is  1/3 
comma  sharp,  and  the  tritone  is  pure  (see  Table  34). 

Table  34.    Silbermann's  1/6  -  Comma  Temperament 

7  1  ,1  2,1  1  4  1  +1  5 

Names  C°  C#"  D"  Eb  a  E"  F  «  F*~     G~«  G#"^  A"  Bd  '  B"  C° 
Cents    0   89   197  305   394  502  590  698  787  895  1003  1092  1200 
M.D.  9.3;  S.D.  9.5 

RomieuS?  adopted  the  1/6-comma  temperament  as  his  "tem- 
perament anacratique,"  showing  its  correspondence  to  the  55- 
division.  A  generation  after  Romieu,  Barca  called  thistempera- 

36Georg   Andreas  Sorge,  Gesprach    zwischen    einem  Musico    theoretico  und 
einem  Studioso  musices  (Lobenstein,  1748),  p.  20. 

3?ln  Memoires  de  l'academie  royale  des  sciences,  1758,  pp.  856 f. 
42 


MEANTONE  TEMPERAMENT 


ment  the  "temperamento  per  coraune  opinione  perfettisimo,"38 
and  showed  that  it  could  be  approximated  by  multiplying  both 
terms  of  the  ratio  81:80  by  6  and  then  tempering  the  fifth  by  the 
mean  ratio  483:482,  which  gives  241:161  for  the  tempered  fifth. 
(A  better  approximation  is  220:147.)  From  additional  references 
to  the  55-division  in  Chapter  VI,  it  would  appear  that  this  method 
of  tuning  was  in  use  for  well  over  a  century.  As  a  system  upon 
which  modulations  might  be  made  to  any  key,  it  was  much  better 
than  the  1/4-comma  meantone  system,  although  inferior  to  most 
of  the  irregular  systems  discussed  in  Chapter  VII. 

Romieu  mentioned  temperaments  of  1/7,  1/8,  1/9,  and  1/10 
commas,  but  did  not  consider  them  sufficiently  important  to  dis- 
cuss. The  1/10-comma  temperament  was  included  among  Mar- 
purg's  many  temperaments. 39  Otherwise  none  of  these  tempera- 
ments has  been  advocated  by  any  of  our  theorists.  They  should 
be  presented,  however,  in  order  to  complete  our  study  of  regular 
temperaments  approaching  equal  temperament  (see  Tables 
35-38).     The  syntonic  comma  has    been  divided    in  each  case. 

Table  35.    1/7  -  Comma  Temperament 

2  +3  4+l  6  18  3  ,2  5 

Names     C°    C#_1    D"    Eb   '    E~~    F  "    F#"    G"    G#"    a"    Bb   7    B~~  C° 
Cents         0     92        198     303     396     501     593      699     791     897     1002    1095  1200 
M.D.   6.3;     S.D.   6.4 


Table  36.    1/8  -  Comma  Temperament 

7  l        u+  3  l        + i  3  1  l  3        u+  1  5 

Names     C°  C#~a  D"  Eb   ■  E"  Fa  F#"  G"  G#"    A-"5  Bb  *  B_1  C° 
Cents         0       95     199    302     397    501     596     699     794    898  1001    1097   1200 
M.D.  4.0;     S.D.  4.1 


38Alessandro  Barca,  "Introduzione  a  una  nuova  teoria  di  musica,  memoria 
prima,"  Accademia  di  scienze,  lettere  ed  arti  in  Padova.  Saggi  scientifici 
e  lettari  (Padova,  1786),  pp.  365-418. 

3^f.W.  Marpurg,  Versuch  iiber  die  musikalische  Temperatur  (Breslau,1776), 
p.  163. 

43 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


With  the  exception  of  some  of  Marpurg's  symmetrical  versions 
of  Neidhardt's  unequal  temperaments,  the  temperaments  shown 
in  Tables  37  and  38  come  closer  to  equal  temperament  than  any 
divisions  that  were  not  practical  approximations  to  it. 


Table  37.    1/9  -  Comma  Temperament 

7  2  14  1  21  8125 

Names     C°    C#"   D"    Eb+3    E"    F"*"5   F#"    g"    G#"    A"  Bb   »  B"5    C° 
Cents        0      97       199      301     398     500     598     700     797     899  1001     1098   1200 
M.D.  2.3;     S.D.   2.4 

Table  38.    1/10  -  Comma  Temperament 

Names     C°    C#"  D"  Eb+"  E"   F+^  F#"s  G"^  G#"  A_T5  Bb+s  B"  C° 
Cents        0      99       200     301     399     500     599     700     798     899    1000    1099  1200 
M.D.   1.2;     S.D.   1.2 


44 


Chapter  IV.    EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


The  first  tuning  rules  that  might  be  interpreted  as  equal  tem- 
perament were  given  by  Giovanni  Maria  Lanfranco. *  As  stated, 
these  rules  were  for  clavichords  and  organs  (Monochordi  & 
Organi),  but  Lanfranco  extended  them  also  to  the  common  stringed 
instruments  of  his  time.  Thus  there  is  none  of  the  confusion  that 
arose  later  when  the  keyboard  instruments  were  tuned  in  one 
manner,  the  fretted  instruments  in  another. 

Lanf  ranco's  essential  rules  concern  the  tempering  of  the  fifths 
and  the  thirds:  the  fifths  are  to  be  tuned  so  flat  "that  the  ear  is 
not  well  pleased  with  them,"  and  the  thirds  as  sharp  as  can  be 
endured.  There  seems  to  be  a  distinction  here:  for  a  fifth  might 
be  tuned  only  slightly  flat  and  the  ear  would  not  then  be  wholly 
pleased  with  it;  but  the  thirds  are  to  be  only  a  shade  less  harsh 
than  those  which  cannot  be  endured  at  all. 

Most  of  Lanfranco 's  contemporaries  still  knew  no  tuning  but 
the  Pythagorean,  with  its  pure  fifths  and  impossibly  sharp  thirds. 
Lanfranco's  rules  seem  to  represent  a  temperament  of  the  Pytha- 
gorean tuning,  rather  than  of  just  intonation.  Equal  temperament 
then  fits  his  directions  excellently.  As  further  evidence,  Lan- 
franco divided  the  notes  to  be  tuned  into  two  classes,  sharps  and 
flats.  As  with  the  meantone  temperament,  the  sharps  included 
F#,  C#,  and  G#,  "although  most  of  these  are  also  common  to  the 
flat  class,  if  not  in  tuning,  at  least  in  playing."  But,  although  the 
flats  proper  included  only  BD  and  ED,  this  class  "occasionally 
needs  in  playing  the  black  keys  F*  (Gb)  and  C#  (D*3)."  As  Kin- 
keldey  says,  "the  enlargement  of  the  major  third,  the  diminution 
of  the  minor  third,  the  equivalence  of  the  notes  C#  and  DD,  F* 
and  GD— these  are  essential  departures  from  his  contempor- 
aries. "2 

Aurelio  Marinati^  honored  Lanfranco  by  inserting  in  his  "ex- 


IScintille  de  musica  (Brescia,  1533),  p.  132. 

2(Xto  Kinkeldey,  Orgel  und  Klavier  in  der  I 
77  f. 

^Somma  di  tutte  le  scienza  (Rome,  1587),  pp.  95-98. 


2(Xto  Kinkeldey,  Orgel  und  Klavier  in  der  Musik  des  16.  Jahrhunderts,  pp. 
77f. 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


ample  of  the  tuning  of  clavichords  and  organs"  a  word-for-word 
account  of  Lanfranco's  system,  complete  even  to  the  title— with- 
out, however,  giving  him  credit  for  it.  Another  plagiarist,  Cerone, 
sufficiently  appreciated  Lanfranco  to  copy  out  his  system  for  the 
benefit  of  organ-builders. 4  At  the  time  when  these  men  were 
writing,  the  meantone  temperament  was  the  recognized  tuning 
norm  for  keyboard  instruments.  It  is  rather  surprising  that 
Cerone  in  particular,  who  had  presented  Zarlino's  2/7-comma 
system  in  detail,  did  not  seem  to  realize  that  there  was  a  con- 
flict between  Zarlino's  flat  and  Lanfranco's  sharp  major  thirds. 

Lodovico  Zacconi^  was  more  astute.  He  presented  no  tuning 
rules  of  his  own,  saying  that  it  is  "better  that  those  who  wish  to 
know  and  to  see  should  look  to  the  source  and  to  the  original 
authors."  For  keyboard  instruments  he  recommended  Aron's 
meantone  temperament.  "As  for  the  other  instruments,  such  as 
the  viole  da  braccio,  viole  da  gamba,  violins,  and  others,  you  can 
look  at  the  end  of  Giovanni  Maria  Lanfranco's  book,  which  indi- 
cates clearly  how  each  one  is  to  be  tuned." 

In  Zacconi's  day  and  long  before  it,  the  fretted  instruments 
were  said  to  have  equal  semitones.  To  Zarlino,  Salinas,  and 
Galilei  this  meant  equal  temperament,  with  all  semitones  equal. 
To  Grammateus  and  Bermudo,  only  ten  semitones  were  equal, 
the  others  being  smaller;  to  Artusi,  and  presumably  also  to  Bot- 
trigari  and  Cerone,  there  were  ten  equal  semitones,  the  other 
two  being  larger.  But,  of  these  three  types  of  temperament- 
equal,  modified  Pythagorean,  and  modified  meantone— only  equal 
temperament  had  both  flat  fifths  and  sharp  thirds  in  addition  to 
equal  semitones.  Therefore,  Zacconi,  writing  only  sixty  years 
after  Lanfranco,  is  practically  saying  that  the  latter 's  rules  rep- 
resent equal  temperament.  In  view  of  the  excellent  tuning 
methods  of  Lanfranco's  immediate  predecessors,  Grammateus 
and  Schlick,  it  is  very  likely  that  Lanfranco  did  intend  equal  tem- 
perament for  all  instruments,  including  clavichords  and  organs. 

Later  writers  who  gave  practical  tuning  rules  for  equal  tem- 
perament were  often  no  more  precise  than  Lanfranco  had  been. 

4See  Kindeldey,  op.  cit.,  p.  80. 

5Prattica  di  musica  (Venice,  1592),  Part  I,  p.  218. 

46 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


Jean  Denis, 6  for  example,  said  nothing  about  the  size  of  the 
thirds.  But  all  the  fifths  are  to  be  lowered  a  trifle  (d'un  poinct), 
"and  all  the  fifths  ought  to  be  tempered  equally."  Denis  may 
even  have  had  some  variety  of  meantone  temperament  in  mind, 
for  he  directed  that  the  tuning  should  begin  with  E*3  and  end  with 
G#.  But  if  his  "toutes"  means  what  it  says,  his  was  equal  tem- 
perament. 

Godfrey  Keller's  tuning  rules  for  harpsichord  or  spinet  were 
widely  circulated,  having  been  reprinted  in  the  appendix  to  William 
Holder's  Treatise  .  . .  of  Harmony  (London,  1731),  and  in  Part  VI  of 
Pierre  Prelleur's  long  popular  Modern  Musick-Master.?  Al- 
though they  can  refer  to  nothing  but  equal  temperament,  they  are 
by  no  means  accurate:  "Observe  all  the  Sharp  Thirds  must  be  as 
sharp  as  the  Ear  will  permit;  and  all  Fifths  as  flat  as  the  Ear 
will  permit.  Now  and  then  by  way  of  Tryal  touch  Unison,  Third, 
Fifth,  and  Eighth;  and  afterward  Unison,  Fourth,  and  Sixth."  It 
is  impossible  for  the  thirds  to  be  very  sharp  and  the  fifths  simul- 
taneously very  flat;  for  in  the  1/5-comma  variety  of  meantone 
temperament,  in  which  the  error  of  the  fifths  and  the  thirds  is 
equal,  the  error  is  not  large.  Keller's  rules  would  read  better 
if  he  had  said  that  the  fifths  were  to  be  only  slightly  flat. 

Barthold  Fritz**  gave  tuning  rules  for  equal  temperament  that 
merited  the  approval  of  Emanuel  Bach,  to  whom  he  had  dedicated 
his  little  book.  Bach  said  that  "in  my  [Fritz's]  few  pages  every- 
thing had  been  said  that  was  necessary  and  possible,  and  that 
would  satisfy  far  more  needs  than  the  sundry  computations  with 
which  many  a  man  has  racked  his  brains;  since  the  latter  method 


^Traite*  de  l'accord  de  l'espinette  (Paris,  1650),  pp.  lOf. 

"*Keller'sbook  had  the  title  A  Compleat  Method  . . .  (London:  Richard  Meares). 
The  British  Museum  has  a  copy  dated  1707,  but  with  a  different  printer. 
The  Library  of  Congress  copy  does  not  contain  the  tuning  rules;  its  copy  of 
the  Prelleur  book  is  the  4th  edition,  dated  1738.  The  British  Museum  has  an 
edition  of  the  latter  dated  1731.  Part  VI  was  printed  separately  with  the 
title  The  Compleat  Tutor  for  the  Harpsichord  or  Spinet,  and  passed  through 
several  editions,  with  various  printers,  in  the  1750's  and  '60's. 

°Anweisung  wie  man  Claviere,  Clavicins,  und  Orgeln,  nach  einer  mechanis- 
chen  Art,  in  alien  zwblf  Tonen  gleich  rein  stimmen  konne,  . . .  (3rd  edition; 
Leipzig,  1780). 

47 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


of  instruction  was  only  for  very  few  people,  but  mine  was  for 
everybody,  the  computers  not  excepted,  because  they  depend  upon 
the  judgment  of  the  ear  as  well  as  the  others. "9 

Fritz's  rules  were  very  simple.  After  going  from  F  to  A  by 
four  tempered  fifths,  he  said,  "I  now  have  the  already  pure  F  as 
a  major  third  to  this  A,  and,  by  touching  the  A  and  by  testing  it 
with  F,  can  hear  whether  it  sounds  sharp  enough  or  so  much  up- 
wards that  the  beats  are  about  the  rapidity  of  eighth  notes  in 
common  time. "10 

Fritz  began  his  tuning  in  the  octave  below  middle  C.  From 
William  Braid  White's  table,  **  the  tempered  F- A  in  this  octave 
will  beat  about  7  times  per  second,  or  over  400  times  in  a  min- 
ute. Even  allowing  for  the  somewhat  lower  pitch  of  the  eighteenth 
century,  Fritz's  eighth  notes  would  be  very  fast,  unless  by  "com- 
mon time"  he  meant  alia  breve. 

Mersennel2  also  gave  a  practical  tuning  hint  for  equal  tem- 
perament when  he  said,  "Certain  people  believe  that  they  can  find 
the  preceding  accord  of  the  equal  semitones  by  beginning  ut,  re, 
mi,  fa,  etc.  on  each  key  of  the  spinet,  or  by  the  number  of  trem- 
blings or  beats  which  the  fifth  and  other  tempered  consonances 
make:  for  example,  the  fifth  beats  once  in  each  second  when  it 
is  tempered  as  it  should  be  (as  much  for  the  organ  as  for  the 
spinet);  whereas  when  it  is  just  it  does  not  beat  at  all."  From 
White's  table,  Mersenne's  rule  would  apply  best  to  the  fifth  D-A 
in  the  octave  above  middle  C,  and  approximately  to  other  fifths 
in  that  vicinity. 

Alexander  Ellis'  practical  rules  for  the  formation  of  equal 
temperament* 3  may  be  paraphrased  as  follows:  If  one  tunes  by 
upward  fifths  and  downward  fourths  within  the  octave  above  mid- 
dle C,  each  fifth  should  beat  once  per  second,  and  each  fourth 

^Ibid.,  Preface  to  2nd  edition. 

IQlbid.,  p.  14. 

11  Piano  Tuning  and  Allied  Arts  (4th  edition;  Boston,  1943),  p.  68. 

l^Harmonie  universelle  (Paris,  1636-37),  Nouvelles  observations  physiques 
&  mathematiques,  p.  20. 

13h.  L.  F.  Helmholtz,  Sensations  of  Tone  (2nd  English  edition,  translated  by 
Alexander  J.  Ellis;   London,  1885),  pp.  489  f. 

48 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


three  times  in  two  seconds.  Ellis  stated  that  if  this  rule  is  fol- 
lowed accurately,  the  error  for  no  pitch  will  be  greater  than  two 
cents.  Again  using  White's  useful  table,  we  find  that  the  mean 
value  of  the  beats  of  the  tempered  fifths  in  the  C-C  octave  is 
1.02  and  of  the  tempered  fourths,  1.47,  proving  that  Ellis'  rule 
is  correct. 

White  himself  "lays  the  bearings"  in  the  F-F  octave, 14  just 
as  Fritz  did.  Since  the  ratio  of  a  tempered  fifth  is  approximately 
3:2,  one  might  suppose  that  he  would  advocate  beating  rates  that 
are  2/3  of  Ellis'  values:  fourths  once  per  second,  and  fifths 
twice  in  three  seconds.  However,  he  recommends  that  the  fifths 
beat  three  times  in  five  seconds,  or  36  times  per  minute,  and 
suggests  setting  a  metronome  at  72,  with  the  bell  ringing  at  every 
second  tick.  Since,  from  his  own  table,  the  mean  value  of  the 
beats  of  his  tempered  fifths  is  .68  rather  than  .60,  he  would  get 
better  results  from  setting  the  metronome  at  80. 

Bossier's  methodic  for  achieving  equal  temperament  is  rem- 
iniscent of  Aron's  method  for  the  meantone  tuning.  Aron,  it  may 
be  remembered,  first  tuned  his  major  third  pure  and  then  tuned 
equally  flat  the  four  fifths  that  were  used  in  constructing  the  major 
third.  Bossier  first  divided  the  octave  by  ear  into  three  equal 
parts— C-E-G#-C.  Then  he  tuned  a  group  of  four  fifths,  as  C-G- 
D-A-E,  slightly  flat,  so  that  the  last  would  give  the  sharp  major 
third  already  found.  The  method  would  be  continued  until  the  en- 
tire octave  was  tuned.  Having  these  first  three  notes  fixed  gave 
him  points  of  reference,  so  that  he  could  never  go  far  wrong. 
But  he  realized  that  the  human  ear  is  fallible,  for  he  recommended 
that  the  tuner  buy  "steel  forks  from  Frankfurt  or  Leipzig  for  all 
twelve  notes." 

Geometrical  and  Mechanical  Approximations 

One  of  the  famous  problems  of  antiquity  was  the  duplication 
of  the  cube.  It  had  been  proved  that  the  construction  of  the  cube 
root  of  2  could  not  be  accomplished  by  Euclidean  geometry,  that 

14Op.  cit.,  p.  85. 

15H.  P.  Bossier,  Elementarbuch  der  Tonkunst  (Speier,  1782),  pp.  xxiv-xxvi. 

49 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


is,  by  compass  and  ruler.  This  is  the  precise  problem  involved 
in  the  solution  of  equal  temperament  by  geometry,  if  Bossier, 
for  example,  had  desired  to  construct  a  monochord  upon  which 
would  be  located  his  C-E-G#-C. 

The  first  sixteenth  century  writer  to  suggest  a  geometrical 
or  mechanical  means  of  solving  equal  temperament  was  Fran- 
cisco Salinas. 16  Let  him  explain  his  method:  "We  judge  this  one 
thing  must  be  observed  by  makers  of  viols,  namely,  that  the  oc- 
tave must  be  divided  into  12  parts  equally  proportional,  which  12 
will  be  the  equal  semitones.  And  since  they  cannot  accomplish 
this  by  the  9th  of  the  6th  book  [the  mean  proportional  construc- 
tion] or  by  any  other  proposition  of  Euclid,  it  will  be  the  task  to 
use  the  instrument  which  we  said  was  called  the  mesolabium, 
invented  (as  they  believe)  by  Archimedes:  by  which  they  will  be 
able  to  obtain  aline  divided  into  as  many  equal  parts  as  they  wish. 
We  have  not  bothered  to  append  the  rule  of  its  construction  here, 
because  mention  is  made  of  its  principle  by  Vitruvius  in  his  9th 
book  on  architecture;  from  whom  and  from  his  expositors  they 
will  be  able  to  obtain  the  method  of  constructing  it:  for  it  is  to 
practical  men  for  framing  most  matters  not  only  useful,  but  well- 
nigh  indispensible." 

The  mesolabium  had  been  previously  advocated  by  Zar lino  for 
constructing  his  2/7- comma  meantone  temperament,  and  later 
Zarlino  was  to  follow  Salinas'  lead  in  recommending  it  for  equal 
temperament.  Hutton  defined  the  word  as  follows:  "Mesolabe, 
or  Mesolabium,  a  mathematical  instrument  invented  by  the  an- 
cients, for  finding  two  mean  proportionals  mechanically,  which 
they  could  not  perform  geometrically.  It  consists  of  three  paral- 
lelograms, moving  in  a  groove  to  certain  intersections.  Its  figure 
is  described  by  Eutocius,  in  his  Commentary  on  Archimedes. 
See  also  Pappius,  Lib.  3. "17 

With  the  aid  of  a  clear  diagram  (Figure  B)  James  Gow^S  has 
explained  the  operation  of  the  mesolabium  as  follows:  "If  AB, 
GH  be  the  two  lines  between  which  it  is  required  to  find  two  mean 

16De  musica  libri  VH,  p.  173. 

^Charles  Hutton,  Mathematical  Dictionary  (new   ed.;   London,  1815). 

l^A  Short  History  of  Greek  Mathematics  (Cambridge,  1884;  reprinted,  New 
York,  1923),  pp.  245 f. 

50 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


proportionals,  then  slide  the  second  frame  under  the  firstandthe 
third  under  the  second  so  that  AG  shall  pass  through  the  points 
C,  E,at  which  the  diameters  of  the  second  and  third  frames,  re- 
spectively, cease  to  be  visible.  Then  CD,  EF  are  the  required 
two  mean  proportionals." 


F    H 


Fig.  B.    The  Mesolabium    (From  James  Gow,  A  Short  History  of  Greek 
Mathematics  [c.  1884]) 

Although  Zarlino  contended  that  the  mesolabium  might  be  used 
for  finding  any  number  of  means,  by  increasing  the  number  of 
parallelograms,  his  diagram  is  for  two  means  only.  Of  course 
for  equal  temperament  or  for  the  1/3-comma  meantone  temper- 
ament, two  means  would  suffice.  But  Salinas  also  advocates  it 
for  an  unlimited  number  of  means,  and  Rossi  would  find  the  thirty 
means  for  Vicentino's  division  by  its  aid.  Mersenne,19  however, 
in  commenting  upon  Salinas' construction  for  equal  temperament, 
said  it  was  incorrect  if  he  intended  to  use  the  mesolabium  for 
more  than  two  means,  because  the  instrument  mentioned  by  Vi- 
truvius  "is  of  no  use  except  for  finding  two  means  between  two 
given  lines."  We  shall  not  attempt  to  pass  judgment  upon  these 
conflicting  opinions,  but  it  would  seem  that  the  difficulty  of  the 
process  would  be  increased  greatly  with  an  increasing  number 
of  means. 

Zarlino^O  has  given  three  methods  by  which  "to  divide  the 
octave  directly  into  12  equal  and  proportional  parts  or  semi- 
tones." The  first  used  the  mesolabium,  as  already  mentioned. 
The  second  used  the  method  of  Philo  of  Bysantium  (second  cen- 
tury, B.C.),  which  consisted  of  a  circle  and  a  variable  secant 


^Harmonie  universelle,  p.  224. 

20Gioseffo  Zarlino,  Sopplimenti  musicali  (Venice,  1588),  Chap.  30. 


51 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


through  a  point  on  its  circumference.  The  third  is  a  variation  of 
the  first,  in  that  the  string-length  for  one  note  is  found  by  the 
mesolabium,  and  then  the  lengths  for  the  other  notes  are  found 
by  similar  proportions. 

Mersenne,21  too,  has  contributed  non-Euclidean  methods  for 
finding  two  geometric  means.  The  first,  ascribed  to  Molthee, 
used  straight  lines  only,  in  the  form  of  intersecting  triangles. 
The  other  method  (Figure  C)  was  furnished  byRoberval  and  used 


Fig 


m    3 

1 

f    A 

\\ 

|C                    ] 

— ^ 

v  u 

\  y 

j 

\^  I 

. 

\ 

.  C .   Rob 

erval's  Method  for  Finding 
Two  Geometric  Mean  Pro- 
portionals  (From  Mersenne's 
HIarmonie  universelle) 

Reproduce 
the  Librai 

>d  by  courtesy  of 
y  of  Congress 

a  parabola  and  a  circle. 22  Kircher23  combined  the  Euclidean 
method  for  finding  one  mean  proportional  with  a  mechanical 
method  for  finding  two  means.  This  latter  is  by  still  another 
method,  consisting  of  two  lines  at  right  angles  and  two  sliding 


21Op.  cit.,  p.  68. 

22ibid,  p.  408. 

23Athanasius  Kircher,  Musurgia  universalis  (Rome,  1650),  I,  207. 

52 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


L -shaped  pieces,  like  carpenters'  squares  (Figure  D).  Accord- 
ing to  Rossi, 24  Kircher's  is  the  method  of  Nicomedes,  and  Rossi 
considered  it  "more  expeditious"  than  others  that  have  been  men- 
tioned. Marpurg25  ascribed  Kircher's  method  to  Plato,  and 
added  methods  by  Hero  and  by  Newton,  together  with  Descartes' 
method  for  finding  any  number  of  mean  proportionals.  Thus  we 
have  more  than  half  a  dozen  geometrical  and  mechanical  methods, 
proposed  particularly  for  constructing  a  monochord  in  equal 
temperament. 


Fig.  D.    Nicomedes'  Method  for  Finding 
Two  Geometric  Mean  Pro- 
portionals   (From  Kircher's 
Musurgia  universalis) 

Reproduced  by  courtesy  of 
the  Library  of  Congress 


Since  these  mechanical  methods  for  finding  two  mean  pro- 
portionals are  rather  awkward,  the  attempt  has  been  made  to  use 
a  satisfactory  ratio  for  the  major  third  or  minor  sixth,  so  that 
the  remainder  of  the  division  could  be  made  by  the  Euclidean 
construction  for  finding  a  single  mean.  Mersenne26  has  given 
two  such  methods.  In  the  second,  which  he  said  is  "the  easiest 
of  all  possible  ways,"  the  just  value  of  the  minor  sixth  (8:5)  is 
used.    By  mean  proportionals,  eight  equal  semitones  are  found 


24sistema  musico,  pp.  95 f. 

25yersuch  liber  die  musikalische  Temperatur,  19.   Abschnitt. 

26Harmonieuniverselle,  p.  69. 


53 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


between  the  fundamental  and  the  minor  sixth,  and  then,  in  like 
manner,  the  remaining  four  semitones  between  the  minor  sixth 
and  the  octave. 

As  can  be  seen  from  Table  39,  this  method  is  not  extremely 
close  to  correct  equal  temperament,  because  the  just  value  of 
the  minor  sixth  is  about  14  cents  higher  than  its  value  in  the  equal 
division.  One  might  have  expected  the  usually  astute  Mersenne 
to  have  chosen  a  tempered  value  in  the  first  place.  The  equally 
tempered  minor  sixth  is  very  nearly  100:63,  as  can  be  readily 
seen  in  Boulliau's  table  given  by  Mersenne,  where  it  bears  ex- 
actly this  value.  If  this  fraction  is  too  difficult  to  work  with, 
27:17  will  serve  almost  as  well,  and  19:12  comes  rather  close 
also.  Any  of  these  other  ratios  would  have  given  a  more  satis- 
factory monochord  than  his.  In  Table  40,  19:12  is  used  for  the 
minor  sixth. 

Table  39.    Mersenne's  Second  Geometrical  Approximation 

Names     CxDxEFxGxA  x  B  C 

Cents       0    102    203    305    407     508    610    712    814    910    1007     1103     1200 

M.D.  2.3;  S.  D.  2.5 

Table  40.    Geometrical  Approximation  (19:12  for  Minor  Sixth) 


Names 

C         x 

D 

X 

E               F 

X 

G 

Cents 

0       99.5 

198.9 

298.4 

397.8       497.3 

596.7 

696.2 

Names 

X 

A 

X 

B 

C 

Cents 

795.6 

896.7 

997.8 

1098.9 

1200.0 

M.D.  .76;  S.D.  .78 

But  we  cannot  be  supercilious  regarding  Mersenne's  other 
practical  method  for  obtaining  two  mean  proportionals.  Mer- 
senne himself  correctly  said,  "It  serves  for  finding  the  mechan- 
ical duplication  of  the  cube,  to  about  1/329  part.  "27  By  the  fa- 
miliar Euclidean  method  he  found  the  mean  proportional  between 
a  line  and  its  double,  subtracted  the  original  line  from  the  mean, 

27lbid.,  p.  68. 
54 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


and  then  subtracted  this  difference  from  the  doubled  line.  The 
length  thus  found  was  the  larger  of  the  desired  means— that  is, 
the  string-length  for  the  major  third.  In  numbers,  this  ratio  is 
(3  -  42):  2,  or  .79289,  which  represents  401.8  cents.  The  result 
is  shown  in  Table  41,  the  remaining  values  being  found  by  mean 
proportionals  as  in  Mersenne's  second  approximation.  This  is 
an  extremely  fine  geometrical  way  to  approximate  equal  tem- 
perament. 

Table  41.    Mersenne's  First  Geometrical  Approximation 


Names 

C          x 

D 

x               E                F 

X 

G 

Cents 

0       100.4 

200.9 

301.3        401.8        501.6 

601.3 

701.1 

Names 

X 

A 

x                      B 

C 

Cents 

800.9 

900.6 

1000.4             1100.2 

1200.0 

M.D.  .30;  S.D.  .32 

Table  42.    Ho  Tchhe'ng-thyen's  Approximation 

Lengths    900   849    802    758    715    677    638    601  570  536  509.5     479     450 

Names         C      C*      D      D*       E      E#     F#      G  G#       A  A*         B        C 

Cents          0      101    200    297    398   493    596    699  791  897  985      1091  1200 

M.D.  4.8;  S.D.  5.8 


Numerical  Approximations 

The  earliest  numerical  approximation  for  equal  temperament 
comes  from  China.  About  400  A.D.,H6  Tchh^ng-thyen  gave  three 
monochords  for  the  chromatic  octave,  with  identical  ratios,  but 
with  the  fundamental  taken  as  9.00,  81.00,  and  100.0  respec- 
tively.28  (string-lengths  are  given  for  the  first  of  these  tables 
only,  since  they  illustrate  the  manner  of  its  formation  better  than 
the  other  two.) 

Table  42  shows  a  remarkable  temperament  for  the  time  when 
it  was  constructed,  comparable  to  the  brilliant  solution  of  the 

^"Maurice  Courant,  "Chine  et  Core'e,"  Encyclopedic  de  la  musique  et  diction- 
naire  du  conservatoire  (Paris,  1913),  Part  1,  Vol.  I,  p.  90. 

55 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


problem  of  equal  temperament  by  Prince  Tsai-yu  over  a  thousand 
years  later.  At  the  time  of  Tchh^ng-thyenthe  Pythagorean  tuning 
was  the  accepted  system  in  China.  If  we  assume  the  calculation 
to  begin  with  the  higher  C  at  450  and  proceed  in  strict  Pythagorean 
manner  to  B#  in  the  lower  octave,  the  B^  will  be  at  888  instead 
of  900.  This  is  12  units  too  short.  Let  us,  therefore,  add  1  unit 
to  600,  the  value  for  G;  2  units  to  800,  the  value  for  D;  3  units 
to  533,  the  value  for  A;  and  so  forth,  along  a  sequence  of  fifths, 
until  we  reach  the  correct  value  for  C  at  900.  Tchh§ng-thyen's 
figures  agree  precisely  with  our  hypothesis. 

A  linear  correction,  such  as  Tchh§ng-thyen  made,  often  pro- 
vides a  good  approximation,  as  we  shall  see  elsewhere  in  this 
chapter.  The  difficulty  with  his  correction  is  that  if  he  had  started 
with  the  lower  C  and  had  continued  until  he  had  reached  the  higher 
B^,  the  latter  would  have  been  only  6  units  too  short  instead  of  12. 
By  adding  10  parts  for  A#,  8  for  G^,  etc.,  he  obtained  pitches  that 
were  much  too  low.  If  he  had  added  12  parts  to  444  for  the  higher 
B^,  the  corrected  length,  456,  would  have  been  at  1177,  instead 
of  1200  cents,  23  cents  flat!  Let  us  consider  the  effect  of  adding 
precisely  half  the  correction  for  each  note.  This  would  work 
well  for  the  odd  semitones,  C  D  E  F#  G#  A#  B#,  as  might  have 
been  expected;  but  the  lower  three  even  semitones,  C^  D#  E^, 
are  then  as  sharp  as  the  higher  odd  semitones  were  flat  before! 
We  shall  have  better  success  if  we  continue  the  series  of  whole 
tones  from  G  to  Fx,  the  latter  at  296  needing  a  correction  of  4.2 
to  make  a  perfect  octave  to  G,  600.5.  Then  the  intermediate 
notes  can  be  given  a  proportional  linear  correction,  which  would 
be  doubled  for  the  three  notes  C*  D#  E#  when  transposed  to  the 
lower  octave.  This  improved  temperament  is  shown  in  Table  43. 
The  greatest  error  is  at  C*. 

Table  43.    H6  Tchh£ng-thyen's  Temperament,  Improved 
Lengths  900        846.6        801         754.8         713         763         635         600.5 


Names 

C 

X 

D               x               E 

F 

X 

G 

Cents 

0 

106 

202          305         403 

503 

604 

701 

Lengths 

566 

534.1 

504.5           475.7 

450 

Names 

X 

A 

x                    B 

C 

Cents 
6 

803 

903 

] 

1004             1101 
M.D.  2.2;   S.D.  2.7 

1200 

EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


The  arithmetical  division  of  the  9:8  tone  into  17:16  and  18:17 
semitones  was  known  to  all  sixteenth  century  writers  through 
Ptolemy's  demonstration  that  Aristoxenus  could  not  have  obtained 
equal  semitones  in  this  way.  But  Cardano  (1501-76)  may  have 
been  referring  to  some  practical  use  of  the  18:17  semitone  when 
he  wrote:  "And  there  is  another  division  of  the  tone  into  semi- 
tones, which  is  varied  by  putting  the  tone  between  18  and  16;  the 
middle  voice  is  17;  the  major  semitone  is  between  17  and  16,  but 
the  minor  between  18  and  17,  the  difference  of  which  is  1/288. 
It  is  surprising  how  the  minor  semitone  should  be  introduced  so 
pleasingly  in  concerted  music,  but  the  major  semitone  never. "29 

The  simplest  way  to  construct  a  monochord  in  equal  tempera- 
ment is  to  choose  a  correct  ratio  for  the  semitone  and  then  apply 
it  twelve  times,  a  construction  that  can  be  performed  very  easily 
by  similar  proportion.  Vincenzo  Galilei^O  must  be  given  the 
credit  for  explaining  a  practical,  but  highly  effective,  method  of 
this  type.  For  placing  the  frets  on  the  lute  he  used  the  ratio  18:17 
for  the  semitone,  saying  that  the  twelfth  fret  would  be  at  the  mid- 
point of  the  string.  He  went  on  to  say  that  no  other  fraction  would 
serve;  for  17:16,  etc.,  would  give  too  few  frets,  and  19:18,  etc., 
too  many.  Since  18:17  represents  99  cents,  17:16,  105  cents,  and 
19:18,94  cents,  Galilei  was  correct  in  his  contention.  But  he  did 
not  give  a  mathematical  demonstration  of  his  method.  It  remained 


Table  44.    Galilei's  Approximation 


Lengths 

100000 

94444 

89197 

84242 

79562 

75142 

70967 

Names 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E 

F 

X 

Cents 

0 

99 

198 

297 

396 

495 

594 

Lengths 

67024 

63301 

59784 

56463 

53326 

50000 

Names 

G 

X 

A 

X 

B 

C 

Cents 

693 

792 

891 

990 

1089 

1200 

M.D.  1.8:   S.D.  3.3 


29Girolamo  Cardano,  Opera  omnia,  ed.  Sponius  (Lyons,  1663),  p.  549. 
30Dialogo  della  musica  antica  e  moderna  (Florence,  1581),  p.  49. 


57 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


for  him  a  proof  by  intuition.  The  string- lengths  in  Table  44  were 
calculated  by  Kepler. 31 

Mersenne32  testified  that  Galilei's  method  was  favored  by 
"many  makers  of  instruments."  The  Portugese  writer  Domingos 
de  S.  Jose  Varella33  gave  a  "way  to  divide  the  fingerboards  of 
viols  and  guitars."  This  is  precisely  Galilei's  method,  and 
Varella  told  how  the  construction  could  be  continued  by  similar 
proportion  after  the  first  18:17  semitone  had  been  formed.  Like- 
wise Delezenne34  showed  that  18:17  is  very  near  the  value  for 
the  correct  equal  semitone,  and  gave  a  geometrical  construction 
for  it  used  by  Delannoy,  the  instrument  maker,  in  placing  the 
frets  upon  his  guitars. 

Two  other  early  nineteenth  century  references  to  what  Gar- 
nault35  called  the  "secret  compass"  of  the  makers  of  fretted 
instruments  were  given  in  his  tiny  and  not  very  trustworthy 
monograph  on  temperament.  The  first  was  from  the  Robet- 
Maugin  Manuel  du  Luthier  (1834),  which  stated  that  if  the  string 
is  2  feet  in  length,  the  first  semitone  will  be  at  a  distance  of  16 
lines  from  the  end;  this  represents  16/2x12x12  =  1/18  the  length 
of  the  string,  thus  giving  18:17  for  the  ratio  of  each  semitone. 

Garnault's  second  reference  was  to  the  Bernard  Romberg 
'cello  method  (1839  ),36  which  he  said  had  been  adopted  byCheru- 
bini  for  use  in  the  Paris  Conservatoire.    Romberg's  directions 


"^Johannes  Kepler,  Harmonices  mundi  (Augsburg,  1619;  edited  by  Ch.  Frisch, 
Frankfort  am  Main,  1864),  p.  164. 

^^Harmonie  universelle,  p.  48. 

33compendio  de  musica  (Porto,  1806),  p.  51. 

34c  E.  J.  Delezenne,  "Memoire  sur  les  valeurs  numeriques  des  notes  de  la 
gamme,"  Recueil  des  travaux  de  la  soci^te*  des  sciences, ...  de  Lille, 
1826-27   p.  49,  note  (a),  and  p.  50. 

35paul  Garnault,  Le  temperament,  son  histoire,  son  application  aux  claviers, 
aux  violes  de  gambe  et  guitares,  son  influence  sur  la  musique  du  xviiie 
siecle  (Nice,  1929),  pp.  29  ff. 

36in  the  German  translation  (original?),  Violoncell  Schull  (Berlin,  1840  [?] ), 
the  directions  are  given  on  page  17;  in  the  English  translation,  A  Complete 
Theoretical  and  Practical  School  for  the  Violoncello,  they  are  omitted. 

58 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


were  much  the  same  as  those  given  previously.  Although  Gar- 
nault  does  not  mention  this,  Romberg  added  that  the  directions 
given  were  for  equal  temperament,  but  the  more  advanced  player 
would  often  make  the  sharped  notes  sharper  and  the  flatted  notes 
flatter  than  these  pitches— another  confirmation  of  the  quasi-Py- 
thagorean tuning  of  instruments  of  the  violin  family. 

These  references  to  the  18:17  semitone  cover  two  and  a  half 
centuries.  It  is  probable  that  they  could  be  brought  much  nearer 
our  own  times  if  the  makers  of  fretted  instruments  were,  given  a 
chance  to  express  themselves.  We  must  accept  Galilei's  method, 
therefore,  as  representing  the  contemporary  practice.  A  player 
on  a  lute  was  not  going  to  bother  with  the  mesolabium  or  with  a 
monochord  on  which  were  numbers  representing  the  successive 
powers  of  the  12th  root  of  2.  But  he  could  place  his  frets  by  a 
simple  numerical  ratio  such  as  18:17,  and  we  are  glad  that  the 
frets  thus  placed  served  their  purpose  so  well. 

Critics  of  Galilei  were  not  slow  to  show  that  the  12th  fret 
would  not  coincide  precisely  with  the  midpoint  of  the  string. 
Passing  by  the  inconveniently  large  numbers  of  Zarlino's  ratios, 
we  come  to  Kepler's  result:  if  the  entire  string  is  100,000  units 
in  length,  Galilei's  12th  fret  will  be  at  50,363  instead  of  50,000. 
As  we  have  already  stated,  his  semitone  has  only  99  cents,  so 
that  the  octave  contains  1188  instead  of  1200. 

There  are  various  ways  of  correcting  the  octave  distortion 
arising  from  the  use  of  the  18:17  semitone.  An  obvious  way  is 
suggested  by  Mersenne's  approximations:  form  only  4  semitones 
with  the  18:17  ratio;  then  apply  Mersenne's  mean-proportional 
method  to  the  remaining  8  semitones.  The  monochord  thus  con- 
structed (Table  45)  is  as  good  as  Mersenne's  first  method. 

Table  45.    Approximation  a  la  Galilei  and  Mersenne 


Names 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E 

F 

X 

G 

Cents 

0 

99 

198 

297 

396 

496.5 

597 

697.5 

Names 

X 

A 

X 

B 

C 

Cents 

798 

898.5 

999 

1099.5 

1200 

M.D.  .67;   S.D.  .71 

59 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


An  even  simpler  correction  uses  linear  divisions  only:  since 
the  length  for  the  12th  fret  is  363  units  too  great,  divide  363  into 
12  equal  parts  and  subtract  30  units  for  the  first  fret,  61  for  the 
second,  91  for  the  third,  etc.  As  is  always  the  case  with  this 
type  of  correction,  there  is  a  slight  bulge  in  the  middle  of  the 
octave,  but  the  largest  error  is  only  1.8  cents. 

The  correction  shown  in  Table  46  lends  itself  well  to  numer- 
ical computation,  since  the  fundamental  and  its  octave  are  in 
round  numbers.  But  in  practice,  with  a  geometrical,  not  a  numer- 
ical, construction,  the  following  would  be  simpler  and  is  even  a 
trifle  better:  if  50,363  be  considered  the  real  middle  of  the 
string,  the  octave  will  be  perfect.  To  make  it  the  middle,  shorten 
the  entire  string  by  twice  the  difference  between  50,000  and 
50,363,  that  is,  by  726.  Then  everyone  of  the  lengths  as  given  by 
Kepler  will  be  diminished  by  726,  and  the  12th  fret,  49,637,  will 
be  the  exact  middle  of  the  string,  99,274.  Note  again  the  slight 
bulge  in  the  middle  of  the  division  (Table  47),  with  the  greatest 
distortion  1.0  cent. 

Table  46.    Galilei's  Temperament,  with  Linear  Correction,  No.  1 


Lengths 

100000 

94414 

89136 

84151 

79441 

74991 

70785 

Names 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E 

F 

X 

Cents 

0 

99.5 

199.1 

298.8 

398.5 

498.3 

598.2 

Lengths 

66812 

63059 

59512 

56160 

52993 

50000 

Names 

G 

X 

A 

X 

B 

C 

Cents 

698.3 

798.4 

898.5 

998.9 

1099.4 

1200 

M.D.  .26;   S.D.  .31 


Table  47.    Galilei's  Temperament,  with  Linear  Correction,  No.  2 


Lengths 

100000 

99274 

88471 

83516 

78836 

74416 

70241 

Names 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E 

F 

X 

Cents 

0 

99.7 

199.4 

299.3 

399.1 

499.0 

599.0 

Lengths 

66298 

62575 

59058 

55737 

52600 

49637 

Names 

G 

X 

A 

X 

B 

C 

Cents 

699.0 

799.0 

899.2 

999.3 

1099.7 

1200 

60 


M.D.  .17;  S.D.  .21 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


The  improvements  upon  Galilei's  tuning  shown  in  Tables  46 
and  47  could  have  been  made  by  practical  tuners.  They  are  better 
divisions  than  many  of  the  numerical  expressions  of  equal  tem- 
perament which  will  be  shown  later.  They  are  better  also  than 
the  temperament  our  contemporary  tuners  give  our  own  pianos 
and  organs.  So  there  is  nothing  more  that  needs  to  be  said,  as 
far  as  practice  is  concerned.  There  are,  however,  several  other 
and  more  subtle  ways  of  improving  Galilei's  tuning  which  we 
should  like  to  mention.  These  are  of  speculative  interest  solely. 

Letus  return  to  the  false  octave  generated  by  the  18:17  semi- 
tone. Mersenne  suggested  that  "if  the  makers  should  increase 
slightly  each  18:17  interval,  they  would  arrive  at  the  justness  of 
the  octave."  The  11th  fret  is  at  53326,  leaving  a  ratio  of  53326: 
50000  for  the  remaining  semitone.  This,  as  its  cents  value  in- 
dicates (111  cents),  is  about  the  size  of  the  just  16:15  semitone. 
Let  us  pretend  that  the  final  digit  in  the  antecedent  is  5,  and  re- 
duce the  ratio  to  2133:2000.  Now  let  us  average  this  semitone 
with  the  eleven  18:17  semitones,  using  the  arithmetical  division 
generally  followed  by  sixteenth  century  writers.  Our  desired 
semitone  is    2000/2133  +  187/18  =  48319.    In  decimal  form   this 

12  51192 

is  .9438779,  as  compared  with  the  true  equal  semitone,  .9438743. 
The  successive  powers  of  this  decimal  would  deviate  more  and 
more  from  those  of  the  12th  root  of  2,  but  even  then  the  octave 
would  be  only  .1  cent  flat. 

Another  way  of  correcting  Galilei's  tuning  is  based  upon  the 
fact  that  his  octave  would  be  12  cents,  that  is,  half  a  Pythagorean 
comma,  flat.  A  somewhat  crude,  but  practical,  manner  of  ad- 
justing the  octave  would  be  to  form  four  18:17  semitones,  from 
C  to  E,  then  take  the  next  five  notes,  F  through  A,  as  perfect 
fourths  to  the  first  five,  and  then  the  two  remaining  notes,  B*3 
and  B,  as  perfect  fourths  to  F  and  F#.  A  satisfactory  monochord 
is  shown  in  Table  48.  Note  particularly  how  much  smaller  its 
standard  deviation  is  than  that  of  Galilei's  actual  tuning. 

As  an  approach  to  a  finer  division  using  Pythagorean  inter- 
vals, let  us  turn  to  Pablo  Nassarre.37 

37Escuela  musica  (Zaragoza,  1724),  Part  I,  pp.  462 f. 

61 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  48.    Galilei's  Temperament  Combined  with  Pythagorean 


Names 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E 

F 

X 

G 

Cents 

0 

99 

198 

297 

396 

498 

597 

696 

Names 

X 

A 

X 

B 

C 

Cents 

795 

894 

996 

1095 

1200 

M.D.  1.5;   S.D.  1.6 

He  had  discussed  equal 
semitones  upon  fretted  instruments,  using  much  the  same  lan- 
guage as  Praetorius,38  to  the  effect  that  a  16:15  diatonic  semi- 
tone contains  5  commas  and  a  25:24  chromatic  semitone  4  com- 
mas, but  that  these  semitones  have  the  peculiarity  that  they  are 
all  equal,  containing  4  1/2  commas.  They  are  obtained  by  a 
linear  division  of  the  9:8  tone  into  18:17  and  17:16  semitones. 
To  place  the  frets,  three  or  four  9:8  tones  are  constructed,  and 
the  distance  between  each  pair  of  frets  divided  equally  to  form 
the  semitones .  Of  course  an  arithmetical  division  of  tones  will 
not  form  precisely  equal  semitones.  Furthermore,  there  is  a 
fairly  large  distortion  for  the  last  semitone  if  the  process  is 
carried  out  through  twelve  semitones.  Of  course,  as  with 
Galilei's  method,  no  single  string  would  have  had  twelve  frets. 
In  Table  49  the  division  is  made  for  the  entire  octave.  The  length 
for  B  was  taken  as  the  arithmetical  mean  between  A^  and  the 
middle  of  the  string. 

Table  49.    Nassarre's  Equal  Semitones 


Names 

C° 

X 

D° 

X 

E° 

(F) 

F#> 

(G) 

Cents 

0 

99 

204 

303 

408 

507 

612 

711 

Names 

G#° 

(A) 

A*0 

(B) 

C° 

Cents 

816 

915 

1020 

1107 

1200 

M.D.  4.2;  S.D.  5.4 


If  Nassarre  had  divided  each  9:8  tone  into  precisely  equal 
semitones  by  a  mean  proportional,  his  errors  would  have  been 
smaller. 

•^Syntagma  musicum,  Vol.  2,  p.  66. 
62 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  50.    Nassarre's  Temperament  Idealized 


Lengths 

100000 

94281 

88889 

83805 

79012 

74494 

70233 

Names 

c° 

X 

D° 

X 

E° 

(F) 

F#° 

Cents 

0 

102 

204 

306 

408 

510 

612 

Lengths 

66216 

62429 

58859 

55493 

52319 

50000 

Names 

(G) 

G*° 

(A) 

A*> 

(B) 

C° 

Cents 

714 

816 

918 

1020 

1110 

1200 

M.D.  3.7;   S.D.  6.7 

It  is  not  particularly  difficult  to  set  down  this  tem- 
perament in  figures,  since  the  square  root  need  be  performed 
only  for  C*,  after  which  a  second  series  of  9:8  tones  can  be 
formed,  starting  with  this  note.  If  B  is  taken  as  the  geometric 
mean  between  A^and  C,  its  length  is  52675,  or  1110  cents,  making 
the  mean  deviation  3.3,  and  the  standard  deviation  4.5.  However, 
for  the  sake  of  an  approximation  to  be  made  in  Table  50,  B  is 
taken  as  the  geometric  mean  between  A^  and  B^,  with  a  relatively 
high  standard  deviation. 

If  we  now  compare  the  cents  values  of  the  temperament  shown 
in  Table  50  with  those  of  Galilei's  tuning,  we  shall  find  that  the 
error  of  the  former  is  opposite  to  and  twice  as  great  as  that  of 
the  latter.  Therefore,  for  every  pair  of  string- lengths,  subtract 
the  smaller  (Nassarre)  from  the  larger  (Galilei),  and  then  sub- 
tract 1/3  the  difference  from  the  larger  number.  The  excellent 
monochord  shown  in  Table  51  results. 


Table  51.    Temperament  a  la  Galilei  and  Nassarre 


Lengths 

Names 

Cents 

Lengths 

Names 

Cents 


100000 

C 

0 
66755 

G 
699.7 


94390 

x 

99.9 

63010 

x 
799.7 


89094 

D 
199.9 
59476 

A 
899.6 


84096 

x 
299.9 
56140 

x 
999.6 


79379 

E 
399.8 
52990 

B 
1099.6 


74926 

F 
499.8 
50000 

C 
1200 


70722 

x 
599.7 


M.D.  .07;   S.D.  .13 


63 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


If  the  idealized  Nassarre  temperament  had  been  extended  one 
more  semitone,  the  string-length  for  the  octave  would  have  been 
49,328.  When  this  number  is  adjusted  with  the  50,363  of  Galilei's 
tuning,  the  octave  proper  to  the  above  temperament  becomes 
50,018  or  1199.5  cents.  Let  us  now  make  the  same  type  of  octave 
adjustment  as  with  the  original  Galilei  tuning,  by  subtracting  18 
from  the  12th  semitone,  and  1  or  2  less  for  each  succeeding 
semitone.  Then  no  length  varies  by  more  than  2  or  3  units  from 
the  correct  value,  that  is,  the  maximum  variation  is  less  than  .1 
cent. 

This  procedure  sounds  somewhat  complicated.  It  is  not  nec- 
essary to  go  through  the  entire  process  three  times,  as  shown 
above,  in  order  to  obtain  the  final  monochord.  The  ratio  for  the 
semitone  will  be  17/9  +  2^2~/3  =  17  +  6/J2~  .    Including  the  octave 

3  27 

correction,  the  formula  for  the  string-length  of  the  nth  semitone 
is:    100,000  /l7  +  6^  \-  3(n-l)  .    Perhaps  it  woukfbe   simpler 


27/2 

after  all  to  stick  to  cube  roots,  especially  when  fortified  with  a 
table  of  logarithms ! 

Johann  Philipp  Kirnberger,39  however,  used  a  very  rounda- 
bout method  of  attaining  equal  temperament,  believing  it  to  be 
simpler  in  practice  than  tuning  by  beating  fifths.  He  showed  that 
the  ratio  10935:8192  closely  approaches  the  value  of  the  fourth 
used  in  equal  temperament.  In  practice  this  value  would  be  ob- 
tained by  tuning  upward  seven  pure  fifths  and  then  a  major  third. 
In  other  words,  if  C°  is  the  lower  note,  E^_1  is  regarded  to  be 
the  equivalent  of  F~^ ,  the  tempered  fourth.  The  basis  for  this 
equivalence  lies  in  the  fact  that  the  schisma,  the  difference  be- 
tween the  syntonic  and  the  ditonic  commas,  is  almost  exactly 
1/12  ditonic  comma,  the  amount  by  which  the  fourth  must  be 
tempered.  The  ratio  given  above  becomes,  in  decimal  form, 
.7491541  .  . . ,  whereas  the  true  tempered  value  is  .7491535  .... 
The  result  is  an  extremely  close  approximation. 


39Die  Kunst  des  reinen  Satzes  in  der  Musik,  2nd  part  (Berlin,  1779),  3rd  Di- 
vision, pp.  179  f. 

64 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


Kirnberger  spoke  of  Euler's  approval  of  his  method,  and  of 
Sulzer's  and  Lambert's  publication  of  it.  Marpurg^O  showed 
that  Lambert's  method,  when  applied  to  an  entire  octave,  will 
differ  for  no  note  by  more  than  .00001.  He  praised  it  as  a  method 
that  needs  no  monochord,  and  believed  that  the  tuning  of  the  just 
intervals  used  in  it  could  be  made  more  quickly  and  accurately 
than  the  estimation  by  ear  of  the  tempering  needed  for  the  fourth 
or  the  fifth.  However,  the  tuning  of  a  pure  major  third  is  so  dif- 
ficult that  Alexander  Ellis  thought  that  better  thirds  can  be  ob- 
tained from  four  beating  fifths  than  by  tuning  the  thirds  directly. 
If  this  be  true,  a  type  of  tuning  in  which  the  essential  feature  is 
a  pure  major  third  could  not  be  very  accurate,  without  consider- 
ing the  labor  of  tuning  eight  pure  intervals  in  order  to  have  only 
one  tempered  interval! 

Kirnberger 's  approximation  for  equal  temperament  was  next 
heard  of  in  England,  where  John  Farey^l  seems  to  have  dis- 
covered it  independently.  In  Dr.  Rees's  New  Cyclopedia^ 2  we 
are  shown  how  Farey's  method  "differs  only  in  an  insensible 
degree"  from  correct  equal  temperament. 

Among  the  monochords  shown  by  Marpurg  is  one  by  Daniel  P . 
Strahle,43  allegedly  in  equal  temperament,  but  actually  unequal, 
as  can  be  seen  in  Table  52.  This  is  a  geometric  construction  of 
a  curious  sort,  for  which  Jacob  Faggot  computed  the  string- 
lengths  by  trigonometry  (see  Figure  E).  In  brief,  it  went  like 
this:  upon  the  line  QR,  12  units  in  length,  erect  an  isosceles  tri- 
angle, QOR,  its  equal  legs  being  24  units  in  length.  Join  O  to  the 
eleven  points  of  division  in  the  base.  On  QO  locate  P,  7  units 
from  Q,  and  draw  RP,  extending  it  its  own  length  to  M.  Then  if 
RM  represents  the   fundamental   pitch  and  PM  its  octave,  the 


^Oyersuch  liber  die  musikalische  Temperatur,  p.  148. 

41uOn  a  New  Mode  of  Equally  Tempering  the  Musical  Scale,"  Philosophical 
Magazine,  XXVII  (1807),  pp.  65-66. 

42ist  American  edition,  Vol.  14,  Part  1,  article  on  Equal  Temperament. 

43«Nytt  pafund,  til  at  finna  temperaturen,  i  stamningen  for  thonerne  pa  cla- 
veretock  dylika  instrumenter,"  Proceedings  of  the  Swedish  Academy,  IV 
(1743),  281-291.  .The  second  part  of  the  article,  "Trigonometriskutrakning," 
appears  under  Faggot's  name. 

65 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


points  of  intersection  of  RP  with  the  11  rays  from  O  will  be  the 
11  semitones  within  the  octave. 

Table  52.    Faggot's  Figures  for  Strahle's  Temperament 


Lengths 

10000 

9379 

8811 

8290 

7809 

7365 

6953 

Names 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E 

F 

X 

Cents 

0 

111 

219 

325 

428 

529 

629 

Lengths 

6570 

6213 

5881 

5568 

5274 

5000 

Names 

G 

X 

A 

X 

B 

C 

Cents 

727 

824 

919 

1014 

1108 

1200 

M.D.  4.8;   S.D.  5.7 


Fig.  E.   Strahle's  Geometrical  Ap- 
proximation for  Equal 
Temperament 
Reproduced  by 
courtesy  of  the 
Library  of  the 
University  of 
Michigan 


It  is  obvious  from  the  construction  that  the  distance  between 
two  consecutive  points  of  division  will  be  greater  near  R  than 
near  P,  and  hence  that,  superficially  at  least,  the  division  will 
resemble  a  series  of  proportional  lines,  as  in  true  equal  tem- 
66 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


perament.  But,  as  Table  52  shows,  there  is  a  large  bulge  in  the 
middle  of  the  octave,  and  F*,  which  should  be  5000^2  =  7071,  is 
distorted  very  greatly.  Now,  if  QR  is  given,  the  points  of  division 
are  functions  of  QO  (or  RO),  but  they  are  also  functions  of  QP. 
It  is  primarily  the  size  of  the  angle  QRP  that  determines  the 
ratios  of  the  string- lengths.  Strahle's  choice  of  7  units  for  QP 
was  unfortunate,  or  the  distortion  would  not  have  been  so  great. 
To  reduce  the  errors  in  this  construction,  let  us  attempt  to 
find  a  value  for  the  angle  QRP  for  which  the  length  for  F#  is 
correct,  V2RM.    Let  A  be  the  midpoint  of  QR  and  B  the  point 

2 
where  OA  cuts  RM;  so  that  BM  is  the  length  for  F- .    Then 

1.  RB  =  42BP  =  a(2RP 

1W2~ 

2.  OQR  =  cos-1  1/4  =  750  31'  . 
By  the  sine  law  and  from  1.  and  2., 

3.  sin  RPQ       12,  or  sin  RPQ  12 

sin  PQR  ~~  RP  fl5/4  RB/1  +  a/2" 

4.  cos  QRP  =  6/RB. 
From  3.  and  4., 

5.  sin  RPQ  =   ^30  cos  QRP       1.1344  cos  QRP 

2(1+^2)  " 

From  2., 

6.  QRP  +  RPQ  =  104°  29'  . 
As  an  approximate  solution  to  5.  and  6., 

7.  QRP  =  33°  36'    and  RPQ  =  70°  53'. 

From  7.,  PQ  =  7.028.  But  this  is  almost  exactly  Strahle's 
figure!  A  check  reveals  that  Faggot  made  a  serious  error  in 
computing  the  angles  QRP  and  RPQ;  so  that  his  value  for  PQ 
was  actually  8.605  rather  than  7.  Table  53  gives  the  correct 
figures  for  Strahle's  temperament. 

67 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  53.    Correct  Figures  for  Strahle's  Temperament 


Lengths 

100000 

9432 

8899 

8400 

7931 

7490 

7073 

Names 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E 

F 

X 

Cents 

0 

101 

202 

302 

401 

500 

600 

Lengths 

6676 

6308 

5955 

5621 

5303 

5000 

Names 

G 

X 

A 

X 

B 

C 

Cents 

699 

798 

897 

997 

1098 

1200 

M.D.   .83;  S.D.   1.00 

It  is,  therefore,  possible  to  achieve  superfine  results  by  fol- 
lowing a  method  essentially  the  same  as  Strahle's.  Although  un- 
aware of  the  possibilities  in  Strahle's  method,  Marpurg  has  col- 
lected many  unusual  and  interesting  temperaments  by  other 
men. 44  Represented  two monochords  by  Schrbter,both  of  which 
are  excellent  approximations  to  equal  temperament  constructed 
from  tabular  differences.  In  the  first  (Table  54),  Schroter  an- 
chored his  column  of  differences  upon  the  notes  of  the  just  minor 
triad,  as  C  ED  G  C,  with  ratio  6:5:4:3.  The  intermediate  notes 
were  obtained  by  arithmetical  divisions.  This  column  of  differ- 
ences is  worth  showing  as  a  monochord  in  its  own  right,  for  the 
method  of  construction  resembles  that  of  Ganassi  and  Reinhard. 
The  mean  deviation  is  about  the  same  as  for  the  Pythagorean 
tuning,  but  the  standard  deviation  is  larger  because  the  semitone 
B-C,  with  ratio  28:27,  is  much  smaller  than  the  others. 

Table  54.    Schroter 's  Column  of  Differences,  No.  1 


Lengths 

54 

51 

48 

45 

42 

40 

38 

36 

Names 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E 

F 

X 

G 

Cents 

0 

99 

204 

317 

435 

520 

608 

702 

Lengths 

34 

32 

30 

28 

27 

Names 

X 

A 

X 

B 

C 

Cents 

804 

906 

1018 

1137 

1200 

M.D.  11.9;   S.D.  15.3 
44yersuch  uber  die  musikalische  Temperatur,  pp.  179  ff. 

68 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


In  Schrbter's  monochord  proper  (Table  55)  the  upper  funda- 
mental (451)  is  the  sum  of  all  the  differences  in  the  above  table, 
save  the  first  number  to  the  left  (54).  Thus  the  lower  fundamental 
(902)  will  be  a  true  octave.  This  monochord  is  a  highly  satis- 
factory approximation  to  equal  temperament. 

Table  55.    Schroter's  Approximation,  No.  1 


Lengths 

902 

851 

803 

758 

716 

676 

638 

Names 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E 

F 

X 

Cents 

0 

100.7 

201.3 

301.1 

399.9 

499.3 

599.7 

Lengths 

602 

568 

536 

506 

478 

451 

Names 

G 

X 

A 

X 

B 

C 

Cents 

700.0 

800.7 

901.1 

1000.8 

1099.4 

1200 

M.D.  .52;   S.D    .59 

Schroter's  column  of  differences  for  the  second  approximation 
(Table  56),  while  also  containing  arithmetical  divisions,  is  con- 
structed more  carefully  than  the  first.  The  minor  thirds  D-F 
and  A-C  have  the  unusual  ratio  19:16  or  297  cents.  All  the  notes 
in  the  tetrachord  G-C  are  pure  fifths  above  the  notes  inthetetra- 
chord  C-F.  Here  the  deviation  is  about  the  same  as  in  Gram- 
mateus'  tuning,  thus  ranking  among  the  best  of  the  irregular  sys- 
tems .4  5 

Table  56.    Schrbter's  Column  of  Differences,  No.  2 


Lengths 

384 

363 

342 

324 

306 

288 

272 

256 

242 

Names 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E 

F 

X 

G 

X 

Cents 

0 

97 

201 

294 

393 

498 

597 

702 

799 

Lengths 

228 

216 

204 

192 

Names 

A 

X 

B 

C 

Cents 

903 

996 

1095 

1200 

M.D.  3.8;  S.D.  4.3 


4^For  Grammateus  see  the  second  part  of  Chapter  VII. 


69 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Schroter's  second  approximation  (Table  57)  is  constructed 
from  the  above  column  of  differences  in  the  same  manner  as  was 
his  first.  Its  deviations,  like  those  of  the  column  of  differences 
upon  which  it  was  based,  are  about  1/3  as  large  as  those  of  the 
first  monochord. 


4843 


600.3 


Lengths 

6850 

6466 

6103 

5761 

5437 

5131 

Names 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E 

F 

Cents 

0 

99.9 

199.9 

299.7 

400.0 

500.2 

Lengths 

4571 

4315 

4073 

3845 

3629 

3425 

Names 

G 

X 

A 

X 

B 

C 

Cents 

700.3 

800.1 

900.0 
M.D.  . 

15; 

999.7 
S.D. 

18 

1099.9 

1200 

Schroter's  success  in  building  up  a  monochord  by  using  well- 
chosen  tabular  differences  suggests  that  the  same  method  be 
applied  to  Ganassi's  tuning,  which  is  rather  similar  to  his  first 
column  of  differences .46  The  sum  of  the  twelve  numbers  of 
Ganassi's  monochord  is  805,  which  is  chosen,  therefore,  for  the 
higher  fundamental.  As  might  have  been  expected,  the  mono- 
chord  (Table  58)  is  very  good. 

Table  58.    Approximation  Based  on  Ganassi's  Monochord 


Lengths 

1610 

1520 

1435 

1355 

1279 

1207 

1139 

Names 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E 

F 

X 

Cents 

0 

99.6 

199.3 

298  6 

398.5 

498.8 

599.2 

Lengths 

1075 

1015 

958 

904 

853 

805 

Names 

G 

X 

A 

X 

B 

C 

Cents 

699.3 

798.8 

898.8 

999.3 

1099.9 

1200 

M.D.  .42;   S.D.  .51 


46see  Chapter  VII  for  Ganassi's  tuning. 
70 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  59.    Monochord  from  Difference  Column,  No.  1 


Lengths 

24 

23 

22 

21 

20              19 

18 

17 

Names 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E               F 

X 

G 

Cents 

0 

74 

151 

232 

316           405 

498 

597 

Lengths 

15 

14 

13 

12 

Names 

A 

X 

B 

C 

Cents 

815 

933 

1062 

1200 
M.D.  18.2; 

S.D.  19.7 

16 


702 


These  rather  amusing  improvements  in  poor  or  fair  tuning 
systems  suggest  that  the  method  be  really  put  to  the  test  by 
choosing  for  the  original  monochord  an  entirely  unsatisfactory 
tuning.  Accordingly,  the  thirteen  numbers  from  12  through  24 
were  chosen  (Table  59).  This  is  so  perverted  a  tuning  system 
that  the  major  third  (E),  the  fourth  (F),  and  the  fifth  (G)  are  pre- 
cisely a  semitone  flat  according  to  just  intonation.  However,  a 
benighted  anonymous  writer  in  the  Mercure  de  France  in  1771 
declared  that  if  the  entire  string  were  divided  into  24  parts,  the 
numbers  12  through  24  would  give  all  the  semitones  .47  Thanks 
to  the  regularity  of  its  construction,  the  deviation  of  this  system 
ranks  it  somewhere  near  the  meantone  tuning! 

In  the  next  monochord  (Table  60)  the  deviation  is  of  the  same 
class  as  that  of  Galilei's  tuning.  Its  higher  fundamental,  210,  is 
the  sum  of  the  numbers  12  to  23  inclusive. 

Table  60.    Monochord  from  Difference  Column,  No.  2 

Lengths  420  397  375  354  334  315              297 

Names              C  x  D  x  E                 F                 x 

Cents                 0  97.5  196.2  296.0  397.7  498.1           599.9 

Lengths  280  264  249  235  222  210 

Names              G  x  A  x  B                   C 

Cents  702.0  803.9  905.2  1005.4  1103  9  1200 

M.D.  1.6;  S.D.  1.9 

47Lionel  de  La  Laurencie,  Le  violon  de  Lullya  Viotti    (Paris,  1924),  Tome  HI 
p.  74. 

71 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


For  our  third  monochord  (Table  61)  we  use  the  lengths  of 
Table  60  as  differences.  Here  the  deviation  is  about  the  same 
as  in  Schrbter's  second  approximation. 

In  the  fourth  and  last  approximation  (Table  62)  the  errors 
have  become  too  small  to  be  recorded  correctly  when  five-place 
logarithms  are  used.  Apparently,  however,  the  deviation  is  again 
about  1/10  that  of  the  previous  monochord. 

Table  61.    Monochord  from  Difference  Column,  No.  3 


Lengths 

7064 

6667 

6292 

5938 

5614 

5289 

4992 

Names 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E 

F 

X 

Cents 

0 

100.1 

200.2 

300.6 

400.9 

501.0 

601.1 

Lengths 

4712 

4448 

4199 

3964 

3742 

3532 

Names 

G 

X 

A 

X 

B 

C 

Cents 

701.0 

800.9 

900.6 
M.D.  .18; 

1000.3 
S.  D.  .21 

1100.1 

1200 

Table  62.    Monochord  from  Difference  Column,  No.  4. 


Lengths 

118758 

112091 

105799 

99861 

94257 

88968 

83976 

Names 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E 

F 

X 

Cents 

0 

100 

200 

300 

400 

500 

600 

Lengths 

79264 

74816 

70617 

66653 

62911 

59379 

Names 

G 

X 

A 

X 

B 

C 

Cents 

700 

800 

900 

1000 

1100 

1200 

Objection  may  be  made  to  Schroter's  approximations,  and  to 
ours  as  well,  on  the  ground  that  the  fundamentals  are  not  round 
numbers  such  as  most  of  the  theorists  used  for  the  representa- 
tion of  equal  temperament.  Let  us  see  whether  we  can  supply 
this  lack.  In  our  third  monochord  (Table  61)  the  length  for  F# 
is  4992.  Let  this  be  our  higher  fundamental.  Add  8  to  it,  and  16 
to  its  double,  the  lower  fundamental.  We  could  then  make  an 
arithmetical  division  to  correct  the  intermediate  numbers.  It  is 
little  more  trouble,  however,  to  take  the  two  left-hand  digits  of 
the  numbers  in  this  same  monochord,  starting  with  the  value  for 

72 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


BD,  40.  Multiply  these  and  those  for  B,  37,  by  .4,  as  16.0,  14.8, 
and  all  the  pairs  of  digits  to  the  left  of  BD  by  .2.  Add  these  num- 
bers to  the  appropriate  numbers  in  Monochord  No.  3,  and  we  have 
a  corrected  monochord,  in  which  the  maximum  error  is  4  units, 
or  about  1  cent  (see  Table  63).  Deviation  is  as  in  the  original 
Monochord  No.  3  (Table  61). 


Table  63.    Monochord  No.  3,  Adjusted 


Lengths 

10000 

9439 

8910 

8411 

7940 

7496 

7075 

Names 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E 

F 

X 

Lengths 

6678 

6302 

5947 

5613 

5297 

5000 

Names 

G 

X 

A 

X 

B 

C 

Fortunately,  it  is  possible  to  make  a  similar  adjustment  of 
our  five-digit  monochord,  No.  4  (Table  62).  Here  we  shall  take 
as  our  lower  fundamental  the  length  for  ED,  99861.  We  need  139 
to  make  a  round  number.  This  is  about  twice  the  length  for  G  in 
Monochord  No.  2.  So  we  divide  the  numbers  in  the  second  mono- 
chord  by  2  or  by  4,  and  add  to  the  appropriate  numbers  in  Mono- 
chord  No.  4.    The  maximum  error  is  6  units,  or  about  1/6  cent. 

A  very  useful  approximation  for  equal  temperament  is  to  ex- 
press all  its  irrational  ratios  as  comparatively  small  fractions. 
Alexander  Ellis^S  has  made  a  table  of  about  150  intervals  within 
the  octave,  which  he  has  represented  by  logarithms,  cents,  and 
ratios,  actual  or  approximate.  Since  all  the  intervals  of  equal 
temperament  are  contained  in  this  table,  it  is  easy  to  list  them 
separately,  as  in  Table  65. 

Table  64.    Monochord  No.  4,  Adjusted 

Lengths  100,000  94,388  89,092  84,093  79,375  74,921       70,716 

Names               C  x                D  x  E  F  x 

Lengths  66,747  62,999  59,462  56,124  52,974  50,000 

Names               G  x                 A  x  B  C 


48H.  L.  F.  Helmholtz,  Sensations  of  Tone,  pp.  453-456. 


73 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  65.    Ellis'  Fractional  Approximations 

Ratios        1       89:84  449:400  44:37      63:50      303:227      140:99      433:289 

Names       Cx  D  xEF  x                G 

Ratios  100:63  37:22  98:55  168:89         2 

Names        x           A  x  B  C 

Charles  Williamson^  has  given  the  material  in  Table  65, 
wrongly  ascribing  it  to  Helmholtz  rather  than  to  Ellis.  By  con- 
tinued fractions  he  himself  found  that  the  majority  of  Ellis'  ratios 
were  correct.  He  objected  to  the  ratio  for  the  major  second 
(449:400),  stating  that  this  interval  can  be  represented  more  ac- 
curately as  the  inversion  of  a  minor  seventh.  The  ratios  for  the 
fourth  (303:227)  and  fifth  (4 33: 289)  he  thought  were  not  sufficiently 
close  either,  and  should  likewise  be  paired.  Ellis'  ratio  for  the 
tritone  (140:99)  was  good,  but  Williamson  preferred  to  use  the 
ratio  for  its  inversion  (99:70),  which  is  no  better. 

Williamson  remarked  that  his  ratio  for  the  tone  (55:49)  oc- 
curs in  Cahill's  patent  for  the  Telharmonium,  and  for  the  tritone 
(99:70)  in  Laurens  Hammond's  patent  for  the  Hammond  Electric 
Organ.  He  had  not  previously  run  across  295:221  or  442:295.  It 
is  interesting  to  note  that  here,  as  in  many  other  instances,  Pere 
Mersenne^O  has  anticipated  the  modern  students  of  temperament. 
Mersenne  stated  that  the  minor  third  of  equal  temperament  is 
approximately  6/5  x  112/113  =  672/565.  Convergents  to  this 
ratio  are  44:37  and  157:132,  the  first  of  these  occurring  in  both 
tables  above.  Mersenne 's  ratio  for  the  major  third  was  5/4  x 
127/126  =  635/504,  convergents  to  which  are  63:50  (as  above) 
and  286:227.  For  the  perfect  fifth  he  gave  the  ratio  32  x  886/887  = 
1329/887,  the  convergent  to  which  is  442:295,  used  by  Williamson. 

Williamson's  reference  to  Hammond's  patent^!  suggests  that 
the  latter 's  ratios  be  examined  in  their  entirety.  (It  must  be 
remembered  that  these  ratios  are  based  on  the  practical  con- 

49 "Frequency  Ratios  of  the  Tempered  Scale,"  Journal  of  the  Acoustical  So- 
ciety of  America,  X  (1938),  135. 

^^Harmonie  universelle,  Nouvelles  observations  physiques  &  mathematiques, 
pTllL 

51L.  Hammond's  Patent,  1,956,350,  April  24,  1934,  Sheet  18. 
74 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


sideration  of  cutting  teeth  on  gears.)  The  difficulty  is  that,  al- 
though it  is  easy  enough  to  reduce  Hammond's  frequencies  to 
ratios  with  no  more  than  two  digits  in  numerator  and  denomin- 
ator, no  one  note  appears  as  unity.  (The  ratios  times  320  are 
the  frequencies  from  middle  C  to  its  octave.)  We  cannot  well 
compare  this  with  Table  65.  If  either  F  or  A,  which  have  the 
simplest  ratios  in  Table  66,  is  given  the  value  of  1,  more  than 
half  of  the  ratios  will  have  three  digits.  Hence  the  composite 
table,  Table  67,  with  decimal  equivalents,  gives  a  better  idea  of 
how  the  three  systems  compare. 


Table 

66 

.    Hammond's  Fractional  Approximations 

Ratios          85:104 

71:82 

67:73 

35:36 

69:67 

12:11          37:32 

Names             C 

X 

D 

x 

E 

F                  x 

Ratios           49:40 

48:37 

11:8 

67:46 

54:35 

85.52 

Names             G 

X 

A 

x 

B 

C 

Table 

67 

.    Compar 

ison  of  Three  Approximations 

Ellis 

Williamson 

Hammond 

Equal  Temperament 

C            200000 

200000 

200000 

200000 

B            188652 

188652 

188697 

188775 

x             178182 

178182 

178182 

178180 

A            168182 

168182 

168182 

168179 

x             158730 

158730 

158677 

158740 

G            149827 

149831 

149796 

149831 

x             141414 

141429 

141414 

141421 

F            133480 

133484 

133499 

133484 

E            126000 

126000 

125942 

125992 

x             118919 

118919 

118881 

118921 

D            112250 

112245 

112207 

112246 

x             105952 

105952 

105928 

105946 

C            100000 

100000 

100000 

100000 

Hammond  has  utilized  some  of  the  same  ratios  as  Ellis  and 
Williamson.  His  tone  G-A  is  55:49;  his  minor  thirds  F-Ab  and 
F#-A  are  44:37;  his  major  third  Eb-G  is  63:50;  his  tritones 
ED-A  and  F-B  are  99:70.    He  had  another  major  third  (Bb-D) 

75 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


with  small  ratio,  73:46,  but  this  is  a  poorer  approximation  than 
63:50.  Note  that  many  of  Hammond's  ratios  are  related  in  pairs, 
but  not  in  the  same  way  as  Williamson's.  The  product  of  the 
ratios  for  F^  and  G#,  F  and  A,  E  and  BD,  and  B  and  D^  is  equal 
to  3:2.  C  and  D  are  not  so  related.  Of  course  the  axis  G  is  ap- 
proximately the  square  root  of  3:2,  and  C*,  the  other  axis,  the 
square  root  of  3:4. 

Let  us  compare  these  three  approximations  with  the  true 
values  for  equal  temperament  to  six  places  (see  Table  67).  For 
Ellis  and  Williamson  these  are  the  decimal  equivalents  of  the 
fractions  as  given.  For  Hammond  the  note  A  was  taken  as  the 
fundamental,  and  his  frequencies  as  given  in  the  patent  have  been 
divided  by  1.1. 

In  our  absorption  with  quasi- equal  temperaments  that  excel 
many  presumably  correct  versions,  we  should  not  neglect  the 
pioneers  who  first  set  down  in  figures  the  monochords  constructed 
upon  the  12th  root  of  2.  The  first  European  known  to  have  formed 
such  a  monochord  is  Simon  Stevin,52  about  1596,  who  said  that 
since  there  are  twelve  proportional  semitones  in  the  octave,  the 
problem  is  to  "find  11  mean  proportional  parts  between  2  and  1, 
which  can  be  learned  through  the  45th  proposition  of  my  French 
arithmetic."  There  he  had  explained  that  mean  proportionals  can 
be  found  by  extracting  roots  of  the  product  of  the  extremes.  He 
now  applied  this  principle,  by  representing  each  semitone  as  the 
12th  root  of  some  power  of  2  (see  Table  68). 


Table  68.    Stevin's  Monochord,  No.  1 


Lengths 

10000 

9440 

8911 

8408 

7937 

7493 

7071 

Names 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E 

F 

X 

Cents 

0 

99.7 

199.6 

300.2 

400.0 

499.6 

600.0 

Lengths 

6675 

6301 

5945 

5612 

5298 

5000 

Names 

G 

X 

A 

X 

B 

C 

Cents 

699.8 

799.6 

900.3 

1000.1 

1099.7 

1200 

52Van  de  Spiegeling  der  Singconst,  pp.  26  ff. 
76 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


In  his  actual  calculations  Stevin  first  computed  notes  7, 4,  and 
5,  that  is,  F^,  Eb,  and  E.  These  involve  no  more  difficult  roots 
than  cubic  and  quartic.  There  is  now  sufficient  material  to  com- 
pute the  remaining  notes  by  proportion,  "the  rule  of  three."  Thus 
the  fifth  note  (7937),  divided  by  the  fourth  (8408),  gives  the  second 
(5440).  This  method  is  much  easier  than  to  extract  the  roots  for 
each  individual  note,  which  runs  into  difficulties  with  the  roots  of 
prime  powers,  as  for  notes  2,  6,  8,  and  12  (C*,  F,  G,  B),  where 
the  12th  root  itself  must  be  extracted.  But  the  method  by  pro- 
portion lacks  in  accuracy,  for  an  error  for  any  note  is  magnified 
in  succeeding  notes.  Even  so,  the  maximum  error  is  only  .4 
cent.  The  deviation  for  Stevin 's  monochord  lies  between  those 
for  Schrbter's  two  monochords. 

Stevin  has  worked  out  a  second  monochord  for  equal  temper- 
ament upon  the  same  principle  as  the  first,  but  with  a  different 
order  of  notes. 53  Here  the  maximum  error,  for  E,  is  1  cent. 
The  fact  that  the  two  monochords  do  differ  indicates  that  pro- 
portion is  not  the  ideal  method  (see  Table  69). 

At  the  same  time  that  Stevin  was  setting  down  the  figures  for 
equal  temperament,  or  perhaps  a  few  years  earlier  (1595),  Prince 
Tsai-yii  in  China  was  making  a  much  more  elaborate  and  careful 
calculation  of  the  same  roots  of  2.^4  We  are  not  told  how  he 
performed  his  calculation,  but,  since  it  is  correct  to  nine  places, 
he  must  have  extracted  the  appropriate  root  for  each  note  sepa- 
rately—and without  the  aid  of  logarithms,  which  were  to  simplify 


Table  69. 

Stevin's 

Monochord, 

No.  2 

Lengths 

10000 

9438 

8908 

8404 

7936 

7491 

7071 

Names 

E 

F 

X 

G 

X 

A 

X 

Lengths 

6674 

6298 

5944 

5611 

5296 

5000 

Names 

B 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E 

53Ibid.,  p.  72. 

54pere  Joseph  Maria  Amiot,  De  la  musique  des  Chinois  (Memoires  concernant 
l'histoire,  . .  .  des  Chinois, "  Vol.  VI  | Paris,  1780]),  Part  2,  Fig.  18,  Plate 
21.  See  also  J.  Murray  Barbour,  "A  Sixteenth  Century  Approximation  for 
IT,"  American  Mathematical  Monthly,  XL  (1933),  69-73. 

77 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


the  problem  so  greatly  for  men  who  attempted  it  a  few  decades 
later.  In  some  cases,  since  the  tenth  digit  will  be  5  or  larger, 
modern  computers  would  round  off  the  number  at  the  ninth  digit 
by  substituting  the  next  higher  digit.  This  is  a  convention  of  our 
mathematics,  intended  to  reduce  the  error  arising  from  rounding 
off  a  number.    Tsai-yli  never  did  this. 

Probably  the  first  printed  solution  of  equal  temperament  in 
numbers  was  made  in  Europe  in  1630,  a  generation  after  Tsai- 
yii's  time,  when  Johann  Faulhaber  solved  a  problem  propounded 
by  Dr.  Johann  Melder  of  Ulm.55  The  problem  was  to  divide  a 
monochord  20000  units  in  length,  so  that  all  intervals  of  the  same 
size  should  be  equal.  Faulhaber  did  not  explain  to  his  readers 
how  he  had  arrived  at  his  result  (Table  71),  presenting  it  rather 
as  a  riddle.  His  monochord  was  for  equal  temperament,  but  con- 
tained several  errors  of  1  in  the  unit's  place.  This  is  the  sort 
of  error  likely  to  occur  when  logarithms  are  used,  and  we  might 
suppose  Faulhaber  had  made  use  of  the  logarithmic  tables  printed 
in  his  book. 


Table  70.    Tsai-yii's  Monochord 


c 

500,000,000 

F 

749,153,538 

B 

529,731 

,547 

E 

793,700,525 

X 

561,231 

,024 

X 

840,896,415 

A 

594,603,557 

D 

890,898,718 

X 

629,960,524 

X 

943,874,312 

G 

667,419,927 

C 

1000,000,000 

X 

707,106,781 

Table  71 . 

Faulhaber 's  Monochord 

Lengths 

20000 

18877 

17817 

16817 

15874 

14982        14141 

Names 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E 

F                x 

Lengths 

13347 

12598 

11891 

11224 

10594 

10000 

Names 

G 

X 

A 

X 

B 

C 

55 Johann  George   Neidhardt,  Sectio   canonis   harmonici  (Ktinigsberg,  1724), 
p.  23. 


78 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


Mersennehas  given  a  number  of  different  tables  of  equal  tem- 
perament. The  most  characteristic,  to  six  places,  was  furnished 
by  Beaugrand,  "very  excellent  geometer. "56  Mersenne  also 
printed  a  table  of  first  differences  for  the  numbers  in  this  mono- 
chord,  to  be  used  in  connection  with  a  method  by  Beaugrand  for 
constructing  the  equal  semitones.  A  comparison  with  Tsai-yu's 
table  shows  this  one  to  be  very  inaccurate,  the  errors  being  much 
larger  than  if  logarithms  had  been  used. 

A  much  more  ambitious  table  was  contributed  by  Galle.57  In 
this  table  the  lengths  were  given  to  eleven  places.  Beside  it 
Mersenne  printed  a  table  with  144,000,000  as  fundamental,  so 
that  the  numbers  might  readily  be  compared  with  those  of  "the 
perfect  clavier  with  32  keys  or  steps  to  the  octave,"  which  had 
been  presented  in  the  book  on  the  organ.  This  table  will  not  be 
included  here,  for  it  seems  likely  that  Mersenne  himself  com- 
puted these  numbers  from  Galleys  larger  table,  by  multiplying 
them  by  .00144.  Of  the  numbers  in  the  table,  the  length  for  D  is 
correct  to  only  five  places.  The  others  agree  fairly  well  with 
Tsai-yii  to  the  ninth  place,  although  there  are  some  slight  diver- 
gences. Beyond  the  ninth  place  no  digits  are  correct.  If  Galle 
was  using  logarithms,  he  made  some  serious  errors  in  interpo- 
lation. But  if  he  was  extracting  roots,  it  is  difficult  to  see  how 
he  failed  to  find  correctly  the  middle  number,  the  length  for  F#, 
which  represents  1011  times  the  square  root  of  1/2.  It  should 
be  ten  units  larger.  The  length  for  E*5  (1011  times  the  fourth 
root  of  1/2)  agrees  neither  with  the  correct  value  nor  with  the 
square  root  of  the  length  for  F#. 

Our  final  table  from  Mersenne^  Was  supplied  by  Boulliau, 
"one  of  the  most  excellent  astronomers  of  our  age."  In  it  he  ex- 
pressed the  string- lengths  for  equal  temperament  in  degrees, 
minutes,  and  seconds.  This  is  equivalent  to  having  a  fundamental 
of  14400  in  decimal  notation,  and  the  errors  should  be  no  greater 
than  for  such  a  table.  However,  the  errors  are  greater  than  in 
Stevin's  four-place  table,  with  a  mean  deviation  of  about  1  cent. 
We  can  only  surmise  how  Boulliau  computed  his  figures.    Evi- 

^"Mersenne,  Harmonie  universelle,  p.  38. 
5*7 Ibid.,  Nouvelles  observations,  p.  21. 
58Ibid.,  pp.  384  f. 


79 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


dently  the  sexagesimal  notation  is  somehow  linked  with  his 
method  of  extracting  the  roots. 

Neidhardt  printed  six-place  tables  in  equal  temperament  from 
Faulhaber,  Mersenne,  and  Biimler,  as  well  as  several  of  his 
own. 59  His  first  original  method  was  to  divide  the  syntonic 
comma  arithmetically,  thus  giving  rise  to  a  twofold  error.  The 
arithmetical  division  makes  little  difference,  but  the  fact  that  the 
syntonic  comma  is  about  two  cents  smaller  than  the  ditonic 
comma  means  that  each  fifth  will  be  about  .2  cent  sharper  than 
in  correct  equal  temperament.  Such  a  division  is  fairly  easy  to 
make,  and,  as  the  cents  values  indicate,  the  errors  are  small. 
The  mean  deviation  is  about  1  cent. 

Later,  Neidhardt^O  Was  to  divide  the  ditonic  comma,  both 
arithmetically  and  geometrically,  the  latter  method  being  genuine 
equal  temperament.  He  contended,  however,  that  the  differences 
between  these  two  methods  were  negligible.  Since  the  greatest 
variation  is  5  units,  in  tables  containing  6  digits,  his  contention 
was  correct.  Note  that  the  numbers  for  the  arithmetical  division 
are  the  larger  throughout  the  table.  The  true  values  come  closer 
to  his  geometrical  division,  but  in  every  instance  lie  between  the 
two. 

Neidhardt's  contemporary,  Jakob  Georg  Meckenheuser,61 
printed  a  table,  "as  computed  in  the  first  Societats-Frucht," 
evidently  the  proceedings  of  some  learned  society.  From  his 
figures,  the  syntonic  comma  is  divided  arithmetically,  as  in 
Neidhardt's  first  monochord.  But  evidently  Meckenheuser's 
division  ran  to  sharps,  for  seven  of  his  notes  were  higher  in 
pitch  than  the  corresponding  notes  in  Neidhardt's  monochord. 
The  higher  C  is  not  a  true  octave,  but  a  B#  tempered  by  a  full 
syntonic  comma,  just  as  his  F  is  really  a  tempered  E#.  The 
ratio  of  these  pairs  of  enharmonic  notes  is  the  schisma,  about 
2  cents.  Thus  even  when  two  temperaments  are  constructed  upon 
the  same  hypothesis  and  both  are  intended  for  equal  temperament, 

59Neidhardt,  Sectio  canonis  harmonici,  p.  32. 

SOlbid.,  p.  19. 

"Ipie  sogenannte  allerneueste  musicalische  Temperatur  (Quedlinburg,  1727), 
p.  51. 

80 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  72.    Beaugrand's  Monochord 


Lengths 

200000       188770       178171 

168178      158740      149829      141421 

Names 

C                 x                D 

x                E                 F               x 

Lengths 

133480      125992       118920 

112245      105945      100000 

Names 

G                x                 A 

x                B                C 

Table  73.    Galle 

's  Monochord 

C 

50,000,000,000 

F             74,915,353,818 

B 

52,973,154,575 

E              79,370,052,622 

X 

56,123,102,370 

x              84,089,641,454 

A 

59,460,355,690 

D             89,090,418,365 

X 

62,996,052,457 

x              94,387,431,198 

G 

66,741,992,715 

C           100,000,000,000 

X 

70,710,678,109 

Table  74.    Boulliau's  Monochord 


Sexagesimal  Notation    Decimal  Notation    The  Same,  20000  as  Fundament 

7200  10000 

7632  10600 

8092  11239 

8573  11907 

9072  12600 

9605  13340 

10179  14138 

10772  14961 

11405  15840 

12110  16819 

12823  17810 

13580  18861 


C    2° 

0' 

0" 

B    2 

7 

12 

x    2 

14 

52 

A    2 

22 

53 

x    2 

31 

12 

G    2 

40 

5 

x    2 

49 

39 

F   2 

59 

32 

E    3 

10 

5 

x    3 

21 

50 

D    3 

33 

43 

x    3 

46 

20 

C    4       0       0 


14400 


20000 


81 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  75.    Neidhardt's  Division  of  Syntonic  Comma 


Lengths 

200000 

188867 

178148 

168229 

158683 

149845 

141344 

Names 

C 

Db 

D 

Eb 

E 

F 

F# 

Cents 

0 

99.1 

200.3 

299.5 

400.6 

499.9 

601.0 

Lengths 

133472 

126041 

118888 

112268 

105898 

100000 

Names 

G 

Ab 

A 

B*> 

B 

c 

Cents 

700.2 

799.3 

900.5 

999.7 

1100.8 

1200 

Table  76.    Neidhardt's  Division  of  Ditonic  Comma 


• 


Arithmetical 

c 

100000 

B 

105948 

X 

112247 

A 

118922 

X 

125994 

G 

133484 

X 

141424 

F 

149831 

E 

158743 

X 

168182 

D 

178182 

X 

188779 

C 

200000 

Geometrical 
100000 
105945 
112245 
118920 
125991 
133483 
141420 
149830 
158739 
168178 
178179 
188774 
200000 


82 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


there  may  be  a  lack  of  agreement  unless  the  process  is  followed 
through  in  exactly  the  same  way  for  both.  If  it  is  true  equal  tem- 
perament, however,  it  does  not  matter  in  what  order  the  notes 
are  obtained,  whether  on  the  sharp  or  the  flat  side  or  mixed  up 
in  anyway  whatever.  In  Table  77,  Meckenheuser's  numbers  have 
been  divided  by  18.  This  tends  to  conceal  his  rather  obvious 
arithmetical  division  of  the  comma:  in  the  original,  every  num- 
ber except  one  (the  length  for  D)  ends  in  zero.  There  the  value 
for  G  had  been  240200000.  This  has  been  corrected  to  240250000, 
since  the  number  should  be  240000000  tempered  by  1/12  x  1/80  = 
1/960. 

Since  the  syntonic  comma  is  much  easier  to  form  than  the 
ditonic,  it  is  easy  to  see  why  it  should  have  been  preferred  as  the 
quantity  to  be  divided.  However,  since  the  ratio  of  the  two  commas 
is  about  11:12,  an  excellent  approximation  for  equal  temperament 
can  be  made  by  tempering  the  fifths  by  1/11  syntonic  comma. 62 
This  was  done  arithmetically  by  Sorge,with  the  results  shown  in 
Table  78.  The  mean  tempering  of  his  fifths  is  1/886,  whence  the 
ratio  of  the  fifth  will  be  .667419962  . .  . ,  instead  of  .667419927  .... 
However,  there  are  larger  errors  for  most  notes,  since  the  tem- 
perament is  not  built  solely  by  fifths,  and  the  temperament  as  a 
whole  is  comparable  to  Neidhardt's  arithmetical  division  of  the 
ditonic  comma. 

Table  77.    Meckenheuser's  Division  of  Syntonic  Comma 

Lengths  200,000,000  188,658,258  178,148,341  168,045,776  158,684,002 

Names                C                        C^  D                      D#                     E 

Cents                  0                     101.0  200.3                 301  3               400.6 

Lengths  149,685,380  141,346,458  133,472,222  125,903,184  118,889,159 

Names               E*                      F^  G                      G#                    A 

Cents              501.6                 600.9  700.2                801.2               900.5 

Lengths  112,147,215  105,899,532  99,894,201 

Names              A*                      B  B^ 

Cents              1001.5               1100.8  1201.8 

62Marpurg,  Versuch  iiber  die  musikalische  Temperatur,  p.  177. 

83 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  78.    Sorge's  Division  of  Syntonic  Comma 


Lengths 

200000 

188775 

178182 

168181 

158743 

149831 

141422 

Names 

C 

C* 

D 

D* 

E 

E* 

F# 

Lengths 

133484 

125994 

118923 

112247 

105948 

100000 

Names 

G 

G* 

A 

A* 

B 

c 

The  impression  is  likely  to  become  quite  strong  as  one  reads 
the  second  half  of  this  chapter  that  equal  temperament  is  nothing 
but  a  mass  of  figures  of  astronomical  size.  Actually,  as  far  as 
the  ear  is  concerned,  a  wholly  satisfactory  monochord  in  equal 
temperament  (or  any  other  tuning  system)  would  be  obtained  from 
the  division  of  a  string  a  meter  long,  marked  off  in  millimeters. 
Mersenne63  gave  such  a  table,  considering  it  more  practicable 
than  the  very  complicated  tables  of  Beaugrand  and  Galle.  It  could 
easily  have  been  constructed  from  one  of  the  more  elaborate 
tables  by  rounding  off  the  numbers  at  three  places.  Oddly,  many 
of  Mersenne's  figures  are  one  unit  too  large.  The  correct  mono- 
chord  is  shown  in  Table  79.  It  is  instructive  to  note  that  the  de- 
viation for  this  monochord  is  larger  than  for  one  of  Marpurg's 
irregular  tunings, 64  and  about  the  same  as  that  for  a  couple  of 
his  other  tunings.  Thus,  to  three  places,  Marpurg's  systems 
would  have  coincided  with  equal  temperament. 

Table  79.    Practical  Equal  Temperament,  after  Mersenne 


Lengths 

1000 

944 

891 

841 

794 

749 

707 

667 

Names 

C 

X 

D 

X 

E 

F 

X 

G 

Cents 

0 

99.8 

199.8 

299.8 

399.4 

500.3 

600.3 

701.1 

Lengths 

630 

595 

561 

530 

500 

Names 

X 

A 

X 

B 

C 

Cents 

799.9 

898.9 

1000.7 

1099.9 

1200 

M.D 

.  .60;  S.D.  .81 

^Harmonie  universelle, 

p.  339. 

^Compare  Marpurg's  Temperaments  E,  B,  and  G  in  Chapter  VH  with  the 
cents  values  of  Table  79. 

84 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


In  1706  young  Neidhardt,  full  of  importance  as  the  author  of 
a  new  book  on  temperament,  Beste  und  leichteste  Temperatur 
des  Monochordi,  held  a  tuning  contest  with  Sebastian  Bach's 
cousin,  Johann  Nikolaus  Bach,  in  Jena. 6 5  Neidhardt  tuned  one 
set  of  pipes  byamonochord  he  had  computed  by  making  an  arith- 
metical division  of  the  syntonic  comma.  Therefore,  although  he 
had  worked  out  this  division  to  six  places,  it  was  about  as  accurate 
as  the  practical  monochord  given  above.  Bach  tuned  another  set 
of  pipes  entirely  by  ear,  and  won  the  contest  handily,  for  a  singer 
found  it  easier  to  sing  a  chorale  in  BD  minor  in  Bach's  tuning 
than  in  Neidhardt's. 

Perhaps  part  of  Neidhardt's  difficulty  lay  in  the  fact  that  it  is 
difficult  to  tune  a  pipe  to  a  string.  Many  years  later,  Adlung 
wrote  that  this  same  Johann  Nikolaus  Bach  had  what  might  be 
called  a  "monopipe"— a  variable  organ  pipe  with  a  sliding  cyl- 
inder upon  which  the  numbers  of  the  monochord  were  inscribed. 66 
Because  of  the  end  correction  for  a  pipe,  this  method  is  likely  to 
be  faulty.  However,  forty  years  before  the  date  of  the  historic 
tuning  contest  in  Jena,  Otto  Gibelius67  described  and  pictured 
just  such  a  pipe,  intended  for  his  meantone  approximation  dis- 
cussed in  Chapter  m.  He  also  gave  an  end  correction,  amount- 
ing to  8/3  the  width  of  the  mouth  of  the  pipe.  In  his  accurately 
drawn  copperplate  (see  Figure  F)  the  width  of  the  mouth  is  11 
millimeters,  making  the  end  correction  about  30  millimeters. 
Since  the  internal  depth  is  about  15  millimeters,  his  rule  cor- 
responds very  closely  to  our  modern  rule  that  the  end  correction 
for  a  rectangular  pipe  is  twice  the  internal  depth.  The  Dayton 
Miller  Collection  now  at  the  Library  of  Congress  contains  several 
specimens  of  the  "tuning  pipe,"  most  of  them  fairly  small. 

Since  the  "tuning  pipe"  was  not  widely  disseminated,  organ- 

65Philipp  Spitta,  Johann  Sebastian  Bach,  trans.  Clara  Bell  and  J.  A.  Fuller- 
Maitland  (2  vols.;  London,  1884),  I,  137 f. 

66Jacob  Adlung,  Anleitung  zu  den  musikalischen  Gelahrtheit  (Erfurt,  1758), 
p.  311.  In  addition  to  the  Neidhardt- Bach  test,  he  described  a  similar  ex- 
perience that  befell  Meckenheuser  in  Riechenberg  vor  Goslar,  where  he 
tried  for  three  days  to  tune  the  organ  by  his  monochord,  but  in  vain.  See 
Jacob  Adlung,  Musica  mechanica  organoedi  (Berlin,  1768),  p.  56. 

67propositiones  mathematico-musicae,  pp.  1-11. 

85 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


bC 
c 
o 
U 


(X  T3 
bD  » 

c  o 

C  T3 
3    O 


« 


o 


bp 


86 


EQUAL  TEMPERAMENT 


ists  tuning  by  the  aid  of  the  monochord  probably  had  no  more 
success  than  Neidhardt  had.  It  is  probable,  however,  that,  like 
Johann  Nikolaus— and  Sebastian,  too— the  organists  did  not  bother 
with  a  monochord  but  relied  upon  their  ears.  Hence  the  tuning 
rules  given  in  the  beginning  of  this  chapter  were  of  the  greatest 
possible  importance  in  practice.  Some  of  them  seem  so  vague 
that  they  would  have  needed  to  be  supplemented  by  oral  direc- 
tions. But  if  we  could  be  sure  that  Mersenne's  rule  that  a  tem- 
pered fifth  should  beat  once  per  second  was  to  have  been  applied 
to  the  fifths  in  the  vicinity  of  middle  C,  we  would  have  as  accurate 
a  rule  for  equal  temperament  as  that  given  by  Alexander  Ellis 
over  two  centuries  later. 

Unfortunately,  the  more  mathematically  minded  writers  on 
equal  temperament  have  given  the  impression  that  extreme  ac- 
curacy in  figures  is  the  all- important  thing  in  equal  tempera- 
ment, even  if  it  is  patent  that  such  accuracy  cannot  be  obtained 
upon  the  longest  feasible  monochord.  This  is  why  Sebastian  Bach 
and  many  others  did  not  care  for  equal  temperament.  They  were 
not  opposed  to  the  equal  tuning  itself ,  and  their  own  tuning  results 
were  undoubtedly  comparable  to  the  best  tuning  accomplished 
today— upon  the  evidence  of  their  compositions,  as  will  be  dis- 
cussed in  the  final  chapter.  But  they  needed  a  Mersenne  to  tell 
them  that  the  complicated  tables  could  well  have  had  half  their 
digits  chopped  off  before  using,  and  that,  after  all,  a  person  who 
tunes  accurately  by  beats  gets  results  that  the  ear  cannot  dis- 
tinguish from  the  successive  powers  of  the  12th  root  of  2. 


87 


Chapter  V.    JUST  INTONATION 


The  seeds  of  just  intonation  had  been  sown  early  in  the  Christian 
era,  when  Didymus  and  Ptolemy  presented  monochords  that  con- 
tained pure  fifths  and  major  thirds  (see  Chapter  II).  But  they 
remained  dormant  during  the  Middle  Ages.  Even  after  the  seeds 
had  sprouted  near  the  beginning  of  the  modern  era,  the  plants 
were  to  bear  fruit  only  occasionally  and  haphazardly. 

Enough  of  our  metaphor.  We  shall  consider  in  this  chapter 
all  12-note  systems  that  contain  some  arrangement  of  pure  fifths 
and  major  thirds.  The  Pythagorean  tuning  may  be  thought  of  as 
the  limiting  form  of  just  intonation,  since  it  has  a  great  many 
pure  fifths,  but  no  pure  major  thirds.  As  the  various  chromatic 
notes  were  added  to  the  scale  during  the  latter  Middle  Ages,  they 
were  tuned  by  pure  fifths  or  fourths  to  notes  already  present  in 
the  scale.  Finally,  fifteenth  century  writers  were  describing 
the  formation  of  a  complete  chromatic  monochord,  using  the 
Pythagorean  intervals.  Such  a  writer  was  Hugo  de  Reutlingen, 
whose  altered  notes  consisted  of  two  sharps  and  three  flats. 
Since  the  more  typical  tuning  has  G#  instead  of  AD,  that  is  shown 
in  Table  80.  Of  course  the  deviation  would  be  the  same  as  for 
Hugo's  tuning.  The  ratio  for  each  diatonic  semitone  is  256:243, 
and  for  the  chromatic  semitone  2187:2048.  Compare  with  these 
ratios  the  relative  simplicity  of  the  ratios  for  Marpurg's  first 
tuning,  the  model  form  of  just  intonation.  (The  lengths  are  very 
much  simpler  also.) 

The  first  known  European  writer  to  break  away  from  the 
Pythagorean  tuning  for  the  tuning  of  the  chromatic  monochord 
was  Bartolomeus  Ramis  de  Pareja.2  Ramisgave  specific  direc- 
tions for  tuning  the  monochord  that  resulted  in  a  system  in  which 
the  six  notes  A^°  -G°  are  joined  by  perfect  fifths,  as  in  the  Pyth- 
agorean tuning,  and  the  remaining  six  notes,  D^-F*-1  ,  also 
joined  by  fifths,  lie  a  comma  higher  than  the  corresponding  notes 

^Flores  musicae  omnis  cantus  Gregoriani  (Strassburg,  1488),  Chapter  EL 

^Musica  practica  (Bologna,  1482);  new  edition,  by  Johannes  Wolf  (1901),  pub- 
lished as  Beiheft  der  Internationale  Musikgesellschaft. 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  80.  Pythagorean  Tuning 

Lengths 

629856 

589824 

559872 

531441 

497664 

472392 

442368 

Names 

C° 

C^° 

D° 

EbP 

E° 

F° 

F*o 

Cents 

0 

114 

204 

294 

408 

498 

612 

Lengths 

419904 

393216 

373248 

354294 

321776 

314928 

Names 

G° 

G#o 

A0 

Bbo 

B° 

C° 

Cents 

702 

816 

906 
M.D.  11.7; 

996 
S.D.  11.8 

1110 

1200 

Names    C 
Cents       0 


C*    D 


Table  81.    Ramis'  Monochord 

bO  -1  0  -W~1_0  h° 


Bt 


B 


92  182  294  386  498  590  702  792  884  996  1088  1200 
M.D.  10.0;  S.D.  10.1 


in  the  Pythagorean  tuning  (see  Table  81).  Thus  there  are  pure 
major  thirds  to  only  the  four  notes  BD-G. 

Montucla,^  writing  a  "history  of  music,"  gave  string-lengths 
for  a  17 -note  tuning,  in  which  twelve  notes  are  the  same  as  in 
Ramis.  The  other  five  extend  the  scale  to  A*-1  and  to  GD'°. 
This  is  a  wholly  useless  extension  because  such  enharmonic  pairs 
as  D^0  and  C*"1  differ  by  the  schisma,  2  cents.  Helmholtz 
was  more  astute  in  constructing  his  24 -note  harmonium  in  just 
intonation,  in  which  the  eight  notes  from  C°  through  C#°  are  joined 
by  fifths;  the  next  eight,  E"1  through  E#_1 ,  furnish  major  thirds 
to  notes  in  the  first  series;  and  the  remaining  eight,  AD+1  through 
A+1,  are  considered  (by  disregarding  the  schisma)  as  equivalent 
to  the  thirds  above  the  notes  in  the  second  series,  i.  e.,  G#~2- 
Gx"2.4 

Ramis'  monochord  does  not  differ  perceptibly  from  the  Pyth- 
agorean tuning.  If  he  had  substituted  DD°  and  all  the  other  Pyth- 
agorean enharmonic  equivalents  of  the  syntonic  notes,  he  would 


3jean  Etlenne  Montucla,  Histoire  des  mathe'matiques  (New  ed.;  Paris,  1802), 
IV,  650. 

4H.  L.  F.  Helmholtz,  Sensations  of  Tone,  pp.  316  f. 
90 


JUST  INTONATION 


have  had  a  monochord  from  E^b0  through  G°,  in  Pythagorean 
tuning.  His  reason  for  making  the  new  division  was  solely  to 
simplify  the  construction  of  the  monochord.  In  his  own  words, 
the  Pythagorean  tuning,  as  given  by  Boethius,  is  "useful  and 
pleasing  for  theorists,  but  tiresome  for  singers  and  irksome  to 
the  mind.  But  because  we  have  promised  to  satisfy  both  [singers 
and  theorists],  we  shall  simplify  the  division  of  the  monochord." 
Later  he  expressed  the  same  idea  in  these  words:  "So  therefore 
we  have  made  all  our  divisions  very  easy,  because  the  fractions 
are  common  and  are  not  difficult." 

Undoubtedly  Ramis'  method  is  easier.  But  if  he  had  desired 
to  obtain  the  equivalent  of  the  Pythagorean  tuning  from  AD  to  C#, 
he  would  have  commenced  his  tuning  with  F#  instead  of  with  C, 
having  notes  with  zero  exponents  from  D°  to  C*°  and  with  -1 
from  G#-1  to  Fx-1 .  On  such  a  monochord,  however,  as  on  the 
usual  Pythagorean  monochord,  the  eight  most  common  thirds 
would  have  been  very  sharp  and  the  four  useless  thirds,  E-A", 
B-E13,  F^-B^,  and  C*-F,  would  have  been  pure.  The  monochord, 
as  Ramis  actually  tuned  it,  has  as  its  four  pure  thirds,  B^-D, 
F-A,  C-E,  and  G-B.  Thus,  although  Ramis  professed  to  be  mak- 
ing his  division  of  the  octave  solely  for  the  sake  of  simplicity, 
the  accidental  result  was  that  several  pure  triads  were  available 
in  keys  frequently  used. 

The  bitter  critics  of  Ramis  in  his  own  day  failed  to  realize 
that  his  tuning  was  just  what  he  had  described:  a  simplified 
equivalent  of  the  Pythagorean  tuning  —  shifted,  however,  by  six 
scale  degrees  to  the  flat  side.  To  them,  any  tampering  with  the 
old  intervals  was  sacrilege.  Many  later  writers,  misled  by 
Ramis'  announced  intentions,  have  stated,  without  examining  his 
monochord,  that  he  had  advocated  temperament.  As  we  have  de- 
fined temperament  and  as  the  word  is  usually  understood,  this  is 
a  serious  misconception.  It  has  even  been  stated  that  Ramis  ad- 
vocated equal  temperament!  Since  Ramis'  book  is  accessible  in 
a  modern  edition,  there  is  no  longer  any  excuse  for  repeating 
such  myths. 

It  must  be  said,  somewhat  sadly,  that  Ramis  was  not  aware 
himself  of   the  peculiar  properties    of   the    monochord   he  had 


91 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


fathered.  For  example,  he  explained  that  although  Eb  does  not 
form  a  major  third  to  B,  D#  is  not  really  needed,  for  the  minor 
triad  B  D  F*  can  be  used  in  making  a  Phrygian  cadence  on  E. 
But  his  interval  B^-E^0  is  slightly  better  than  the  Pythagorean 
thirds,  Ab°-C°  and  Eb°-G°,  that  were  acceptable  to  him! 

Ramis  must  have  been  a  good  practical  musician.  Although 
his  system  would  not  now  be  called  a  temperament,  we  might  do 
well  to  take  him  at  his  own  evaluation  and  hail  him  as  the  first 
of  modern  tuning  reformers. 

Corroboration  of  Ramis'  tuning  system  is  found  in  an  inter- 
esting anonymous  German  manuscript  of  the  second  half  of  the 
fifteenth  century,  Pro  clavichordiisfaciendis,  which  Dupont^  ran 
across  in  the  Erlangen  University  Library.  Starting  with  the 
note  B,  C  is  to  be  a  just  semitone  (16:15)  higher,  E  a  perfect 
fourth,  G  a  just  minor  sixth  (8:5),  etc.  A  succession  of  pure 
fifths  on  the  flat  side  extends  to  Gb,  below  which  there  is  a  just 
major  third  (5:4),  E^b,  and  the  monochord  is  completed  by  add- 
ing BbD,  the  fifth  above  Ebb!  The  complete  monochord  is  shown 
in  Table  82. 

The  deviation  for  this  tuning  is  almost  precisely  the  same  as 
for  that  of  Ramis,  and  it  too  contains  many  pure  fifths  and  sev- 
eral pure  thirds.  However,  it  has  one  peculiar  feature  as  Du- 
pont  has  presented  it.  In  every  other  tuning  system  we  have  ex- 
amined, there  has  been  an  uninterrupted  succession  of  notes 
connected  by  fifths  from  the  flattest  to  the  sharpest.  In  the  Pyth- 
agorean and  other  regular  tuning  systems,  such  as  the  meantone, 
the  wolf  fifth  would  be  very  flat  or  sharp,  and  in  the  irregular 
systems  there  would  be  other  divergences.  But  the  note  names 
persisted,  usually  from  Eb  to  G*  inclusive. 

Table  82.    The  Erlangen  Monochord 

Names    C°    Db°  Ebb+1Ebo     E"1     F°     Gb°     G°      Ab°   Bbb+1Bb°      B_1       C° 
Cents       0      90    202    294     386    498     588     702     792    904    996     1088     1200 

M.D.  10.3;   S.D.  10.5 


^Wilhelm  Dupont,  Geshichte  der  musicalischen  Temperatur  (Erlangen,  1935), 
pp.  20-22. 

92 


JUST  INTONATION 


But  in  the  Erlangen  monochord  there  is  no  D  or  A,  and  the 
notes  that  Dupont  has  given  as  their  enharmonic  equivalents, 
E^b  and  B^b,  are  not  in  a  fifth -relation  with  any  other  notes  in 
the  monochord.  Therefore  it  seems  obvious  that  the  anonymous 
writer  intended  these  notes  to  be  D°  and  A0,  each  of  which  is 
higher  by  the  schisma  than  E^b*1  and  Bbb+1  respectively.  Then 
the  notes  that  are  pure  thirds  above  D°  and  A0  will  be  F*-1  and 
C^"1  ,  notes  that  continue  the  fifth-series  from  B"1.  It  would 
then  be  immaterial  whether  to  call  the  semitone  between  G  and 
A  by  the  name  AD°  or  G*  * ,  since  either  would  complete  the 
scale  correctly.  The  original  writer,  by  the  way,  had  not  named 
the  black  keys,  merely  designating  the  semitone  between  C  and 
D  as  the  first,  between  D  and  E  as  the  second,  between  F  and  G 
as  the  third,  and  between  G  and  A  as  the  fourth.  In  renaming 
some  of  the  black  keys,  therefore,  we  are  not  violating  his  in- 
tent, but  rather  confirming  it.  The  revised  monochord,  with 
schismatic  alterations,  is  shown  in  Table  83. 

These  two  pre-sixteenth-century  tunings,  the  one  in  Spain 
and  the  other  in  Germany,  are  sufficient  indication  of  the  trend 
of  men's  thinking  with  regard  to  consonant  thirds.  Lodovico 
Fogliano,"  half  a  century  later  than  Ramis,  offered  no  apologies 
for  using  the  5:4  ratio  for  the  major  third.  But  he  was  not  con- 
tent to  present  ordinary  just  intonation.  Realizing  that  D°  formed 
an  imperfect  fifth  below  A"1 ,  he  advocated  D-1  as  a  consonant 
fifth.  This  in  turn  led  him  to  BD°  as  a  pure  major  third  below 
D-1,  as  well  as  the  Bb+1  as  third  below  D°.  But  he  said  the 
"practical  musicians"  used  only  one  key  each  for  D  and  B*3, 
"neither  right  nor  left,  but  the  mean  between  both."  "Such  a 
mean  D  or  BD,  moreover,  is  nothing  else  than  a  point  dividing 
the  proportion  of  the  comma  into  two  halves." 

Table  83.    Erlangen  Monochord,  Revised 

Names     C°    C*"1  D°      E*    E_1      F°      F^G0      G*"  A°       Bb°     B_1        C° 
Cents       0      92     204     294     386    498     590    702     794    906    996     1088     1200 

M.D.  10.0;   S.D.  10.1 


6Musica  theorica  (Venice,  1529),  fol.  36. 


93 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


To  obtain  the  mean  proportional  by  geometry,  Fogliano  used  the 
familiar  Euclidean  construction,  and  appended  a  figure  to  show  how 
the  division  was  to  be  made.  This  alteration  of  pure  values,  he  said 
is  "what  they  [the  practical  musicians]  call  temperament."  Here 
is  the  germ  of  the  meantone  temperament,  which  his  countryman 
Aron  had  described  in  its  complete  format  aboutthis  same  time. 

For  the  sake  of  showing  monochords  in  just  intonation  from 
the  early  sixteenth  century,  there  are  set  down  here  three  mon- 
ochords after  Fogliano,  first  with  his  one  pair  of  D's  and  B^'s, 
then  with  the  second  pair,  and  finally  with  the  mean  D  and  B*3. 
The  first  monochord  (Table  84)  is  the  best,  having  two  groups  of 
four  notes  each  with  like  exponents.  The  second  monochord 
(Table  85)  would  have  had  the  same  deviation  as  the  first  if  it  had 
had  F#_1  (in  place  of  F#"2)  as  third  above  D°.  (This  is  Mar- 
purg's  first  monochord,  Table  96.)  The  monochord  with  the  two 
meantones  (Table  86)  ranks  between  the  first  two.  If  Fogliano 
had  formed  three  meantones,  including  one  on  F#,  the  deviation 
would  be  slightly  less  than  for  the  first  monochord.  The  result 
is  given  in  Table  87. 


Table  84.    Fogliano's  Monochord,  No.  1 


Lengths 

3600 

3456 

3240 

3000 

2880 

2700 

2592 

2400 

Names 

C° 

c*-2 

D"1 

Eb+i 

E"1 

F° 

F#-2 

G° 

Cents 

0 

70 

182 

316 

386 

498 

568 

702 

Lengths 

2304 

2160 

2025 

1920 

1800 

Names 

G,_2 

A"1 

Bbo 

B_I 

C° 

Cents 

772 

884 

996 

1088 

1200 

M.D.  21.3;   S.D.  23.6 


94 


JUST  INTONATION 


Table  85.    Fogliano's  Monochord,  No.  2 


Lengths 

3600 

3456 

3200 

3000 

2880 

2700 

2592 

2400 

Names 

C° 

c#-2 

D° 

Eb+1 

E"1 

F° 

F*-2 

G° 

Cents 

0 

70 

204 

316 

386 

498 

568 

702 

Lengths 

2304 

2160 

2000 

1920 

1800 

Names 

Gfr. 

A-1 

Bb+i 

B"1 

C° 

Cents 

772 

884 

1018 

1088 

1200 

M.D.  25.0;   S.D.  26.7 


Table  86.    Fogliano's  Tempered  Just  Intonation 


Lengths 

3600 

3456 

|_3220j 

3000 

2880 

2700 

2592 

2400 

Names 

C° 

c*-2 

D-i 

Eb+i 

E"1 

F° 

F*« 

G° 

Cents 

0 

70 

193 

316 

386 

498 

568 

702 

Lengths 

2304 

2160 

[2012.5] 

1920 

1800 

Names 

G#-2 

A"1 

Bb+I 

B"1 

C° 

Cents 

772 

884 

1007 

1088 

1200 

M.D.  23.2;  S.D.  24.7 
Table  87.    Fogliano's  Tempered  Just  Intonation,  Revised 


Lengths 

3600 

3456 

[3220] 

3000 

2880 

2700 

[2576] 

Names 

C° 

c#-2 

1 
D    2 

Eb+1 

E"1 

F° 

F#    2 

Cents 

0 

70 

193 

316 

386 

498 

579 

Lengths 

2400 

2304 

2160 

[2012.5] 

1920 

1800 

Names 

G° 

G#-l 

A'1 

Bb+i 

B"1 

C° 

Cents 

702 

772 

884 
M.D.  21.3; 

1007 
S.D.  22.3 

1088 

1200 

95 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Martin  Agricola'  resembled  Ramis  in  his  tuning  ideas.  He 
gave  a  monochord  in  which  the  eight  diatonic  notes,  including  B*3, 
were  joined  by  pure  fifths,  as  in  the  Pythagorean  tuning.  Then 
he  directed  that  the  interval  from  B  to  the  end  of  the  string  be 
divided  into  ten  parts,  with  C*  at  the  first  point  of  division,  D# 
at  the  second,  and  G*  at  the  fourth.  Then  F*  was  to  be  a  pure 
fourth  to  C*.  Thus  these  black  keys  were  given  syntonic  values, 
and  the  whole  monochord  is  made  up  of  notes  with  0  and  -1  ex- 
ponents (see  Table  88).  Ramis'  monochord  is  slightly  better  than 
Agricola' s,  with  a  ratio  of  6:6  for  the  number  of  fifths  in  each 
group,  in  place  of  8:4. 

Table  88.    Agricola 's  Monochord 

Names    C°    C^D0      D*"1  E°      F°     F*"1  G°     G*-1   A0     Bb°      B°         C° 
Cents       0      92    204    296    408    498    590    702    794    906    996    1110    1200 

M.D.  10.3;  S.D.  10.5 

It  will  be  observed  that  the  better  of  Fogliano's  untempered 
monochords  has  more  than  twice  the  deviation  of  Ramis'.  Thus 
it  might  be  thought  that  Fogliano  had  been  unfortunate  in  his 
choice  of  intervals.  Quite  the  contrary.  The  most  symmetric 
form  of  just  intonation  for  the  series  ED-G*  has  four  notes  with 
the  same  exponent,  followed  by  four  more  with  exponents  that 
are  one  less.  Of  the  remaining  four  notes,  two  would  have  +2 
and  two  would  have  -2  as  exponents.  This  is  precisely  Fogliano's 
second  monochord,  if  we  should  substitute  F^"1  in  it.  Fogliano's 
first  monochord  has  the  exponential  pattern  1,4,4,3,  which  is  just 
as  satisfactory.  (That  is,  the  tuning  contains  one  note  with  ex- 
ponent +  1,  4  with  0  and  -1  exponents,  and  3  with  -2.)  The  diffi- 
culty, therefore,  is  inherent  in  just  intonation  itself,  as  will  be 
discussed  further  a  bit  later. 

Salomon  de  Caus^  was  one  of  several  mathematicians  of  the 
early  seventeenth  century  who  were  interested  in  just  intonation. 

'"De  monochorea'  dimensione,"   in  Rudimenta    musices    (Wittemberg,   1539) 

°Les  raisons  des  forces  mouvantes  avecdiverses  machines  (Francfort,  1615) 
Book  3,  Problem  III. 

96 


JUST  INTONATION 


If  we  follow  his  directions,  we  obtain  the  monochord  shown  in 
Table  89.  Here  there  are  three  groups  of  four  notes  each  with 
the  same  exponent  —  the  most  symmetric  arrangement  of  all. 
The  deviation  is  appreciably  less  than  in  Fogliano'  s  arrangement. 
Johannes  Kepler^  gave  some  genuine  tuning  lore  together 
with  an  elaborate  discussion  of  the  harmony  of  the  spheres.  His 
two  monochords  in  just  intonation  (Tables  90  and  91)  are  identi- 
cal except  that  the  second  has  a  G*  in  place  ofanAD.  Since  Kep- 
ler had  five  notes  with  zero  exponents  in  both  monochords,  the 
deviation  for  his  systems  is  lower  than  most  that  have  been  pre- 
sented in  this  chapter. 

Table  89.    De  Caus's  Monochord 


Names    C°    C^D"1    D#_2  E" 


?#-2  no 


1#"«    A-l 


B 


bo 


B_1 


Cents   0   70  182  274  386  498  568  702  772  884  996  1088  1200 

M.D.  17.7:  S.D.  20.1 


Table  90.    Kepler's  Monochord,  No.  1 


Lengths 

1620 

1536 

1440 

1350 

1296 

1215 

1152 

Names 

C° 

c"-» 

D° 

Eb+i 

E_1 

F° 

jr#-i 

Cents 

0 

92 

204 

316 

386 

498 

590 

Lengths 

1080 

1024 

960 

900 

864 

810 

Names 

G° 

G*+i 

A0 

Bb+i 

B"1 

C° 

Cents 

702 

794, 

906 

1018 

1088 

1200 

M.D.  14.0;  S.D.  15.8 


^Harmonices  mundi,  p.  163. 


97 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Although  Marin  Mersenne  was  a  zealous  advocate  of  equal 
temperament  in  practice,  he  took  pains  to  present  literally  doz- 
ens of  tables  in  just  intonation.  He  repeated,  among  others, 
Kepler's  two  monochords  shown  in  Tables  90  and  91,  together 
with  tables  for  keyboards  with  split  keys.  Four  of  his  monochords 
(Tables  92-95)  are  worth  including  here,  as  evidence  of  the  va- 
riety that  is  possible  in  a  type  of  tuning  that  is  ordinarily  thought 
to  be  fixed  and  uniform.^  None  is  as  good  as  either  of  Kep- 
ler's two. 


Table  91.    Kepler's  Monochord,  No.  2 


Lengths 

100000 

93750 

88889 

833333 

80000 

75000 

71111 

Names 

C° 

c#-i 

D° 

Eb+i 

E_1 

F° 

f'-' 

Cents 

0 

92 

204 

316 

386 

498 

590 

Lengths 

66667 

62500 

60000 

56250 

53333 

50000 

Names 

G° 

Ab+i 

A0 

Bb+i 

B"1 

C° 

Cents 

702 

814 

906 
M.D.  14.0; 

1018 
S.D.  15.8 

1088 

1200 

Table  92.    Mersenne's  Spinet  Tuning,  No.  1 


Lengths 

3600 

3375 

3240 

3000 

2880 

2700 

Names 

C° 

Db+1 

D"1 

Eb+1 

E_1 

F° 

Cents 

0 

112 

182 

316 

386 

498 

Lengths 

2400 

2250 

2160 

2025 

1920 

1800 

Names 

G° 

Ab+1 

A-1 

Bbo 

B*1 

C° 

Cents 

702 

814 

884 
M.D.  17.7 

996 
S.D. 

1088 
20.1 

1200 

lOMersenne,  Harmonie  universelle,  pp.  54,  117  f. 
98 


2531  1/4 
nb+i 


610 


JUST  INTONATION 


Table  93.    Mersenne's  Spinet  Tuning,  No.  2 


Lengths 

3600 

3456 

3200 

3072 

2880 

2700 

2592 

2400 

Names 

C° 

C#-2 

D° 

D*-2 

E"1 

F° 

F#"2 

G° 

Cents 

0 

70 

204 

274 

386 

498 

568 

702 

Lengths 

2304 

2160 

2025 

1920 

1800 

Names 

Gf-2 

A"1 

Bb 

B"1 

Co 

Cents 

772 

884 

996 

1088 

1200 

M.D.  21.3;  S.D.  23.6 

Table  94.    Mersenne's  Lute  Tuning,  No.  1 

Names    C°     Db+1  D"1    Eb+1  E"1     F°      Gb+1  G°      Ab+1A_1    Bb+1     B"1      C° 
Cents       0       112     182    316    386    498     610    702    814    884    1018    1088    1200 

M.D.  21.3;  S.D.  23.6 

Table  95.    Mersenne's  Lute  Tuning,  No.  2 

Names     C°     Db+1  D°      Eb+1  E"1    F°      Gb+1  G°     Ab+1  A"1  Bb+1     B_1      C° 
Cents       0       112    204     316    386    498     610    702    814    884    1018    1088    1200 

M.D.  17.7;   S.D.  20.1 

Table  96.    Marpurg's  Monochord,  No.  1 


Lengths 

900 

864 

800 

750 

720 

675 

640 

Ratios 

24/25 

25/27 

15/16 

24/25 

15/16 

128/135 

15/16 

Names 

C° 

c*-2 

D° 

Eb+1 

E"1 

F° 

F*'1 

Cents 

0 

70 

204 

316 

386 

498 

590 

Lengths 

600 

576 

540 

500 

480 

450 

Ratios 

24/25 

15/16 

25/27 

24/25 

15/16 

Names 

G° 

G*-2 

A"1 

Bb 

B"1 

C° 

Cents 

702 

772 

884 

1018 

1088 

1200 

M.D.  21.3; 

S.D.  23.6 

99 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  97.    Marpurg's  Monochord,  No.  3 

Names     C°    C#_2D°      Eb  *   E^1      F°     F#_1    G°     G^2    A0     Bb°       B"1       C° 
Cents       0      70    204     306    386    498     590    702    772    906    996     1088     1200 

MD.  19.3;   S.D.   22.0 

Table  98.    Marpurg's  Monochord,  No.  4 

Names     C°    C^D-1    Eb+1  E _1     F°    F#~2G0     G^A"1    Bb  +1     B_1      C° 
Cents        0       70    182    316    386    498    568    702    772    884     1018     1088     1200 

M.D.  25.0;  S.D.  26.7 

Note  that  Mersenne's  first  spinet  tuning  (Table  92)  has  flats 
for  its  black  keys  and  the  second  tuning  (Table  93)  has  sharps 
except  for  BO.  The  first  tuning  is  constructed  exactly  the  same 
as  deCaus's  tuning  (Table  89),  except  that  it  begins  a  major  third 
lower,  with  Gb  instead  of  B°.  Mersenne's  first  lute  tuning  (Ta- 
ble 94)  differs  from  his  first  spinet  tuning  (Table  92)  at  only  one 
pitch  (BD+1  instead  of  B130),  but  that  is  enough  to  increase  its  de- 
viation to  that  of  the  second  spinet  tuning  (Table  93).  The  second 
lute  tuning  (Table  95),  although  differing  from  the  first  spinet 
tuning  (Table  92)  at  two  places,  has  the  same  deviation. 

Friedrich  Wilhelm  Marpurg,  *■  who  wrote  brilliantly  about 
temperament  140  years  after  Mersenne,  included  four  mono- 
chords  in  just  intonation.  The  second  of  these  was  Kepler's 
first,  and  need  not  be  repeated  here.  The  other  three  are  shown 
in  Tables  96-98.  In  each  of  them  the  notes,  according  to  their 
exponents,  are  grouped  into  four  classes.  The  first  may  be  con- 
sidered the  model  form  of  just  intonation,  the  ideal  form  of  Fog- 
liano's  second  monochord  (Table  85). 

Opelt  has  shown  two  monochords  in  just  intonation  from  Rous- 
seau's Dictionary. *•*  The  first  (Table  99)  was  by  Alexander 
Malcolm,  whose  linear  improvement  upon  just  intonation  is  to  be 
found  in  Chapter  VII.  This  is  the  same  as  Kepler's  second  mon- 
ochord (Table  91),  transposed  a  fifth  lower. 


11Versuch  liber  die  musikalische  Temperatur,  pp.  118,  123. 

12F 

100 


12F.  W.  Opelt,  Allgemeine  Theorie  der  Musik  (Leipzig,  1852),  p.  46. 


JUST  INTONATION 


Rousseau  tried  to  "improve"  upon  this  tuning  by  substituting 
other  just  pitches  in  place  of  D*3"1"1,  F*"1 ,  and  B"0,  with  very  un- 
satisfactory results,  since  his  division  of  the  major  tone  of  204 
cents  was  into  semitones  of  70  and  134  cents!  This  monochord 
(Table  100)  is  the  reverse  of  Marpurg's  fourth  (Table  98),  with 
semitones  paired  in  contrary  motion,  when  Rousseau's  A*3*1  is 
made  to  coincide  with  Marpurg's  G*~2. 


Table  99.    Malcolm's  Monochord 

Names     C°    Db+1  D  °     Eb+1  E"1     F°      f""1  G°    Ab+1  A"1  Bb°      B"1       C° 
Cents        0      112    204     316    386    498     590    702    814    884    996     1088     1200 

M.D.  14.0;   S.D.  15.8 

Table  100.    Rousseau's  Monochord 

Names     Cu    C#"2  D°     Eb+1    E"1    F°      F^"2   G°     Ab+l  A"1    Bb+1    B"1       C° 
Cents       0      70    204     316    386    498     568     702    814     884     954     1088     1200 

M.D.  25.0;  S.D.  26.7 

Table  101.    Euler's  Monochord 

Names     C°     C^2  D°     D#_2    E_1      F°      F*"1  G°     G*~2A_1    A*"2     B_1       C° 
Cents       0      70    204     274     386    498     590    702     772    884     976     1088     1200 

M.D.  17.1;   S.D.  20.1 

Table  102.    Montvallon's  Monochord 

Names     C°     C#_1  D°     E^1    E"1     F°     F#_1   G°      G#_1   A"1    Bb°      B"1       C° 
Cents       0      92    204     316    386    498     590    702     794    884    996     1088     1200 

M.D.  12.0;  S.D.  13.3 


101 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  103.    Romieu's  Mono-chord 

Names     C°     C#"2D°     Eb+1  E"1     F°     F^G0     G^A"1    B°°      B"1       C° 
Cents       0       70    204     316    386    498     590    702    772    884    996     1088     1200 

M.D.  17.7;   S.D.  20.1 

Euler's  monochord  ran  entirely  to  sharps.  **  However,  it  has 
the  same  symmetric  grouping  of  its  notes  as  de  Caus's  (Table 
89),  only  transposed  a  fifth  higher. 

Montvallon's  monochord,  given  by  Romieu,14  follows  a  more 
familiar  order  in  the  selection  of  notes  than  Euler's  did  (see  Ta- 
ble 102). 

Romieu  himself  contributed  an  example  (Table  103)  of  a 
"syst^me  juste."  15  it  has  a  somewhat  more  complicated  pattern 
than  Euler's  (Table  101),  but  the  same  deviation. 

Theory  of  Just  Intonation 

In  the  foregoing  pages  there  have  been  presented  more  than 
twenty  different  monochords  in  authentic  just  intonation,  i.  e., 
with  pure  fifths  and  major  thirds.  Their  mean  deviations  have 
varied  from  10.0  to  25.0.  And  yet  each  has  a  right  to  be  called 
just  intonation!  This  great  divergence  can  be  explained  by  math- 
ematics. Let  us  consider  first  a  monochord  in  the  Pythagorean 
tuning.  Its  mean  deviation  is  11.7.  A  Pythagorean  chromatic 
semitone,  as  C°-C*°,  is  114  cents;  the  diatonic  semitone,  as 
C#°  -D° ,  90.  Hence  the  deviation  for  the  pair  of  semitones  is  24 
cents.  When  the  just  semitones  are  used,  the  chromatic  semi- 
tone, C-C*"1,  is  92  cents;  the  diatonic,  C#_1-D°,  112.  The 
deviation  for  the  pair  of  just  semitones  is  20  cents,  or  4  cents 
less  than  for  the  pair  of  Pythagorean  semitones.  Therefore  the 
substitution  of  each  just  note  reduces  the  deviation  by  4/12  or 
.3  cent. 

**A.  F.  H'aser,  "Uber  wissenschaftliche  Begriindung  der  Musik  durch 
Akustik,"  Allgemeine  musikalische  Zeitung,  1829,  col.  145. 

14"Me"moire  theorique  &  pratique  sur  les  systemes  tempe're's  de  musique," 
Memoires  de  l'acade'mie  royale  des  sciences,  1758,  p.  867. 

15Ibid.,  p.  865. 

102 


JUST  INTONATION 


But  the  sixth  note  to  be  altered  around  the  circle  of  fifths  is 
adjacent  to  the  first  note  to  have  been  altered,  and  therefore  the 
total  deviation  is  unchanged.  The  same  is  true  for  the  seventh 
note.  The  eighth  note  lies  between  two  notes,  each  sharper  by 
the  syntonic  comma.  Therefore,  when  it  too  is  raised,  the  syn- 
tonic semitones  already  present  are  changed  to  Pythagorean 
semitones,  and  the  deviation  is  increased  by  .3  cent.  This  proc- 
ess continues  until  all  twelve  notes  have  been  raised  by  a  comma, 
and  the  monochord  is  again  in  Pythagorean  tuning.  If  we  call  the 
number  of  notes  with  -1  exponent  nx ,  and  with  0  exponent  n2 ,  the 
following  formula  gives  the  mean  deviation: 


3D,    =  29  +    n ,    -  6    + 


6  -    n,    -  6 


The  minimum  deviation  of  10.0  cents  occurs  when  (nx  ,n2)  =  (5,7), 
(6,6),  or  (7,5).  Thus  Ramis'  monochord  (Table  81)  with  6,6  is 
one  of  the  three  best  possible. 

When  there  are  notes  with  three  different  exponents,  the 
change  of  a  single  note  may  cause  a  greater  change  in  the  devi- 
ation than  was  possible  with  two  exponents  only.  Suppose  a  mon- 
ochord contains  the  notes  C°  C#_1  D"1,  the  total  deviation  being 
18  cents  for  the  two  semitones.  When  C*  "2  is  used,  the  devia- 
tion becomes  42  cents,  an  increase  of  24  cents.  But  if  the  notes 
had  originally  been  C  C#_1  D°,  the  change  to  C*"2  would  in- 
crease the  deviation  from  20  cents  to  64  cents,  that  is,  by  44 
cents,  or  two  commas.  Again,  the  deviation  of  the  two  semitones 
C#_1  D°  Eb+1  is  24  cents;  with  D'1  it  is  44  cents,  an  increase 
of  20  cents. 

Thus  when  a  note  is  changed  by  a  comma,  the  change  in  the 
mean  deviation  may  be  1/3  (as  before)  or  6/3  or  11/3  or  5/3 .  A 
much  more  complicated  formula,  therefore,  is  needed  to  express 
the  deviation  with  the  three  exponents.  If  we  call  the  number  of 
notes  with  -1  exponent  nx ,  with  0,  n2 ,  and  with  +1,  n3 ,  the  mean 
deviation  is  given  by  the  formula: 


103 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


3  D3  =  23  +    ru  -  6    +    n3  -  6    + 


6  -    rii  -  6 


6  -    n3  -  6 


7(k2  -  kx)  +  5(k4  -  k3),  where  kx  =  the  larger  of  n2  and  (7  -  nx), 
k2  =  the  smaller  of  7  and  (12  -  ni),  k3  =  the  larger  of  n2  and 
(5  -  ni),  and  k*  =  the  smaller  of  5  and  (12  -  m).  The  terms  con- 
taining the  k's  are  zero  whenever  k2  <ki  and  ki  ^k3. 

Let  us  now  compute  the  deviations  for  two  of  the  tunings 
shown  on  previous  pages.  Mersenne's  first  spinet  tuning  (Table 
92)  has  for  its  (n1,n2,n3)  the  numbers  (4,4,4).  Here  ki  =  n2  =  4, 
Ko  —  •  y  Ko  —  n  2  —  +■  j  ■K-4  —  **  • 

3  D3  =  23  +  2  +  2  +  0  +  0  +  7x3  +  5x1  =  53.   D3  =  17.7. 
For  Mersenne's  second  spinet  (Table  93)  or  first   lute  tuning 
(Table  94)  the  exponential  numbers  are  (4,3,5). 

3  D3=  23 +  2  +  1  +  0  +  0  +  7x4  +  5x2  + 64.    D3=  21.3. 

When  there  are  four  different  exponents,  there  is  a  very  an- 
alogous formula  for  the  deviation: 


3  D4  =  23  -  nx  +  \n1  +  n2  -  6  |+|n4  -  6  |  + 


6  -    n.  -6 


6  -In!  +  n2  -  6 


+  7(k2  -  kx)  +  5(k4  -  k3)  +  7(L2  -  LJ  +  5(L4  -  L3  ), 


where  kx  =  the  larger  of  n3  and  (7  -  nx  -  n2),  k2  =  the  smaller  of 
7  and  (12  -  nx  -  n^,  k3  =  the  larger  of  n3  and  (5  -  nx  -  n2),  k4  = 
the  smaller  of  5  and  (12  -  nx  -  n2);  L,=  the  larger  of  n2  and 
(7  -  nx),  L2  =  the  smaller  of  7  and  (12  -  nj,  L3  =  the  larger  of 
n2  and  (5  -  nt),  and  L4  =  the  smaller  of  5  and  (12  -  nj.  The 
terms  containing  the  k's  and  L's  are  zero  whenever  k2  <  kx, 
k4  <  k3,  L  2  <  Li,  and  L4  <  L3   . 

As  examples,  let  us  compute  the  deviation  for  two  of  Mar- 
purg's  tunings.  His  first  tuning  (Table  96)  is  the  model  form  of 
just  intonation,  with  (2,4,4,2)  for  its  (n1,n2,n3,n4).  Here  kx  =  4, 
k2  =  6,  k3  =  4,  k4  =  5,  Li  =  5,  L2  =  7,  L3  =  4,  and  L4  =  5.  Hence 
3  D4=  23  -2  +  0  +  4  +  1  +  0  +  7x2  +  5x1  +  7x2  +  5x1  =  64. 
D4  =  21.3.    Marpurg's  third  tuning  (Table  97)  has  for  its  expo- 


nents (2,3,6,1).    Here  k2  =  6,  k3  =  7, 

The  deviation: 


i   -  6,  k4   -  5,  Lx  -  5, 


L2  =  7,  L3  =4,  L4  =  5. 


k, 
3D 


=  23-2  +  1  +  5  +  0 


104 


JUST  INTONATION 


+  0  +  7x1  +  0  +  7x2  +  5x2  =  58.    D4=  19.3. 

With  all  these  complex  mathematical  formulas  before  us,  we 
are  likely  to  forget  that  we  are  ostensibly  studying  a  form  of 
tuning  that  to  many  people  is  a  sort  of  ideal  system.  It  is  not 
likely  that  any  sane  person  would  advocate  so  perverted  a  tuning 
as  that  represented  by  (5,1,1,5),  with  a  mean  deviation  of  43.3 
cents.  But  the  systems  that  have  been  shown  on  the  previous 
pages  have  all  been  advocated  by  various  writers,  and  they  show 
great  variety  in  their  construction  and  almost  as  great  a  variety 
in  their  deviations,  ranging  from  the  10.0  of  Ramis  to  the  25.0 
of  Fogliano's  second  or  Rousseau's  or  Marpurg's  fourth.  The 
model  form,  Marpurg's  first,  with  a  deviation  of  21.3,  comes 
nearer  the  maximum  than  the  minimum.  We  shall  speak  again 
of  just  intonation  in  the  final  chapter.  Let  us  close  this  chapter 
with  a  double  paradox:  there  is  no  such  thing  as  just  intonation, 
but,  rather,  many  different  just  intonations;  of  these,  the  best  is 
that  which  comes  closest  to  the  Pythagorean  tuning. 


105 


Chapter  VI.    MULTIPLE  DIVISION 


If  a  keyboard  instrument  is  not  in  equal  temperament,  its  intona- 
tion can  be  improved  by  a  judicious  increase  in  the  number  of 
notes  in  the  octave.  The  first  reference  to  split  keys  came  from 
Italy,  where  before  1484  the  organ  of  St.  Martin's  at  Lucca  had 
separate  keys  for  E^  and  D*  and  also  for  G*  and  A*3.*  At  this 
same  time,  Ramis^  noted  that  split  keys  were  being  used  in  Spain, 
but  objected  to  having  separate  keys  for  A*3  and  G*  and  for  F* 
and  G*3,  on  the  ground  that  this  would  be  mixing  the  chromatic 
with  the  diatonic  genus.  From  Germany  came  further  evidence 
of  the  divided  keyboard  from  Arnold  Schlick,^  who  referred  to 
an  organ  constructed  at  the  turn  of  the  sixteenth  century  "that 
had  double  semitones  on  manual  and  pedal . . .  which  were  called 
half  semitones  or  'ignoten.'" 

There  are  frequent  references  to  multiple  division  during  the 
sixteenth  and  seventeenth  centuries,  chiefly  by  Italian  theorists. 
Jean  Rousseau^  in  1687  deplored  the  fact  that  the  French  clave- 
cins did  not  have  the  "doubles  feintes"  common  inltaly,  and  con- 
sequently had  "mauvais  effets  dans  les  Tons  transposez."  But 
the  split  keys  must  have  been  very  common  in  Germany  during 
the  latter  part  of  the  seventeenth  and  beginning  of  the  eighteenth 
centuries,  if  we  may  judge  by  the  copious  references  to  "sub- 
semitonia"  by  Werckmeister  and  his  successors.  Buttstett,  it  is 
true,  said  in  1733  that  the  sub-  and  supersemitonia  were  "mehr 
curieux  als  practicabel."5  But  six  years  later,  in  Holland,  van 
Blankenburg  was  to  show  u't  Gesnede  Clavier"  with  three  extra 


Iwilhelm  Dupont,  Geschichte  der  musicalischen  Temperatur,  p.  45. 

^Musica  practica,  Tract.  2,  Cap.  4. 

^Spiegel  der  Orgelmacher  und  Organisten  (Maintz,  1511),  Chap.  8.  Reprinted  in 
Monatshefte  fiir  Musikgeschichte,  1869. 

4Traite~  de  la  viole  (Paris,  1687),  p.  50. 

^Johann  Heinrich  Buttstett,  Kurze  Anfiihrung  zum  General-Bass  (2nd  edition; 
Leipzig,  1733),  p.  20. 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


keys,  as  well  as  an  "Archicymbalam"  with  eighteen  notes  in  the 
octave.  6 

Handel  played  on  English  organs  with  split  keys.'  Father 
Smith's  Temple  Church  organ  in  London,  constructed  in  1682-83, 
had  the  same  pairs  of  divided  keys  as  the  Lucca  organ,  G#-Ab 
and  D#-Eb,  and  so  did  Durham  Cathedral.  The  organ  of  the 
Foundling  Hospital  (1759)  had  an  ingenious  mechanism  by  which 
Db  and  Ab  could  be  substituted  for  C#  and  D#,  or  D#  and  A#  for 
Eb  and  Bb,  thus  increasingthe  compass  to  sixteen  notes,  without 
increasing  the  number  of  keys. 

Many  of  the  sources  said  nothing  about  the  tuning  of  the  extra 
notes,  and  we  can  freely  assume  that  whatever  variety  of  mean- 
tone  temperament  was  used  for  the  twelve  regular  notes  was  ex- 
tended both  clockwise  and  counterclockwise  around  the  circle 
(or,  rather,  spiral)  of  fifths.  More  interesting  to  us  are  the  sys- 
tems that  represent  just  intonation,  as  extended  to  the  enharmonic 
scale.  We  have  already  noted  that  Fogliano  (1529)  had  felt  the 
need  for  two  D's  and  two  Bb's,  to  ensure  just  triads,  but  was 
willing  to  settle  for  a  mean  D  and  a  mean  B*3.  But  van  Blanken- 
burg,  mentioned  above,  included  both  pairs  of  notes  inhisArchi- 
cymbalam,  and  so  did  almost  all  of  the  men  whose  systems  will 
be  described  below. 

The  "enharmonic  genus"  of  Salinas"  was  one  of  the  earliest 
and  best  of  these  systems.  Although  it  contained  twenty-four 
notes,  it  had  nothing  in  common  with  a  real  enharmonic  scale 
composed  of  quarter  tones.  It  is  just  intonation  extended  to  seven 
sharps  and  six  flats.  In  tabular  form  it  would  appear  as  shown 
in  Table  104. 

Observe  that  all  the  notes  in  the  right  diagonal  are  duplicated 
on  the  left,  a  comma  lower.  Thus  it  is  possible  to  play  all  major 
triads  from  Gb  through  G*,  and  all  minor  triads  from  Eb  through 
E*.  Mersenne's  "parfait  diapason"  ^  is  based  upon  Salinas'  sys- 
tem, with  the  addition  of  seven  more  notes,  or  thirty-one  in  all 

"Quirinus  van  Blankenburg,  Elementa  musica  (The  Hague,  1739),  p.  112. 
'Helmholtz,  Sensations  of  Tone,  p.  434. 
8De  musica  libri  VII,  p.  122. 
^Harmonie  universelle,  p.  338. 

108 


MULTIPLE  DIVISION 


(see  Figure  G).    These  would  be  joined  to  Table  104  on  the  left 
side,  as  shown  in  Table  105. 


Table  104.    Salinas'  Enharmonic  Genus 

A#-2  E#-2  B#-2 


,#-i        n¥-i        n#-i 


»#-! 


yfl-1  Cf-1  Qff-1  DP-1  A 

D°  A°  E°  B°  F*° 

Bb+i        F+i  c+i  G+i  D+1 

Gb+2      Db+2        Ab+2        Eb+2        Bb+2 

Gb+3 


f-1 


Fig.  G.   Mersenne's  Keyboard  with  Thirty-One  Notes  in  the  Octave 
(From  Mersenne's  Harmonie  universelle) 

Reproduced  by  courtesy  of  the  Library  of  Congress 


Table  105.    Mersenne's  Addition  to  Salinas'  System 
A" 


Fu 


E"1      B"1 
G° 


Ab+i    Eb+i 


This  is  not  a  particularly  clever  addition.  Note  that  Mersenne 
did  not  have  a  C°.  Furthermore,  for  the  sake  of  symmetry, 
there  should  have  been  Db+1  in  the  lowest  line  of  Mersenne's 
additional  notes,  Bbb+2  ,  Fb+2  ,  and  Cb+2  in  the  line  below  it,  and 
C*~2,  G*"2,  and  D*~2  in  the  line  above  the  highest  line,  or  a  total 
of  thirty-nine  notes. 

109 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


The  praiseworthy  thing  about  Mersenne's  addition  is  that  it 
recognized  the  need  for  having  more  pairs  of  notes  differing  by 
a  comma.  Imperfect  as  his  scheme  was,  it  would  be  much  more 
useful  than  the  34-note  keyboard  of  Galeazzo  Sabbatini,  given  by 
Kircher. 10  There  were,  as  usual  with  Kircher,  many  errors  in 
the  figures,  and  an  erratic  manner  of  naming  the  notes.  The  ac- 
tual notes  of  Sabbatini' s  keyboard  are  shown  in  Table  106. 

Table  106.    Sabbatini's  Keyboard 


Cx-3           Gx-3      Dx-3           Ax 

A*"2 

E#_2                B#-2              [F#-2] 

F#-i 

C*"1           G*-1       D#_1 

D° 

A0 

E°             Bu 

Bb* 

F+1 

C+1             G+1 

|Gb+2]        Db+2 

Ab+2 

Eb+2 

3       Bbb+3 

Fb+3 

cb+3 

gbb+3 

cbb^  Gbb^       Dbb^  Abb 

Ebbb+5     Bbbb+5       [pbb+5] 
Dbbb-^ 


Except  for  the  three  notes  in  brackets  which  have  been  sup- 
plied, this  is  a  beautifully  symmetric  scheme.  But  how  different 
from  that  of  Salinas!  Here  there  are  no  notes  differing  by  the 
syntonic  comma,  with  the  result  that  no  major  triad  based  on  a 
note  in  the  diagonal  on  the  right  will  have  a  pure  fifth,  and  there 
will  be  a  similar  series  of  defective  minor  triads.  With  this  in- 
tonation it  is  not  even  possible  to  supply  a  missing  note  by  its 
enharmonic  equivalent,  because  no  pair  of  notes  differs  by  the 
ditonic  comma  either.  The  most  characteristic  small  interval 
in  it  is  the  great  diesis  of  42  cents,  as  between  A#~2  and  BD+1, 
whereas  A^"1,  needed  as  the  fifth  of  the  D*  triad,  lies  almost 
half  way  between  these  two  notes,  22  cents  higher  than  A*"2  and 
20  cents  lower  than  B^*1.  Other  small  intervals  of  little  use 
contain  28,  14,  and  8  cents.    This,  then,  is  an  example  of  just 

l^Athanasius  Kircher,  Musurgia  universalis,  I,  460. 
110 


MULTIPLE  DIVISION 


intonation  carried  to  an  absurd  end. 

Doni's  three-manual  organ  keyboard* *  (abacus  Triharmon- 
icus)  was  more  elaborate  than  any  system  previously  described, 
with  sixty  keys  in  the  octave,  but  with  only  thirty -nine  distinct 
pitches.  The  lowest  keyboard  was  the  Dorian,  then  the  Phrygian, 
and  finally  the  Lydian.  The  arrangement  of  the  notes  on  each 
keyboard  was  identical,  and  the  keyboards  were  tuned  a  major 
third  apart,  so  that  the  Dorian  E,  the  Phrygian  C,  and  the  Lydian 
A^  were  the  same  pitch.  The  tuning  was  largely  just,  as  can  be 
seen  from  Table  107,  which  represents  seventeen  of  the  twenty 
notes  on  one  keyboard. 


Table  107.   Doni's  Keyboard 

2  JT#-2  C#-2  Q#-2  D^2 

D-1         A"1  E"1  B"1 


*b° 


C°  G° 


Gb+i  Ab+i  Eb+i 

This  arrangement  is  somewhat  lacking  in  symmetry,  and  the 
additional  three  notes,  which  were  real  quarter  tones,  were  of  no 
use  except  to  illustrate  the  scales  of  the  Greeks,  this  being  one 
of  the  uses  of  the  organ.  The  enharmonic  notes  were  formed,  as 
Didymus  formed  his,  by  an  arithmetical  division  of  the  syntonic 
semitone,  16:15,  into  32:31  and  31:30  quarter  tones.  2 

The  nineteenth  century  was  particularly  rife  with  proposals 
to  increase  greatly  the  number  of  notes  in  the  octave.  Many  of 
the  instruments  upon  which  the  inventors  practiced  their  ingen- 
uity were  harmoniums,  intended  for  experimental  purposes  only. 
One  of  the  more  modest  was  Helmholtz's,  already  mentioned  in 
Chapter  V,  with  only  twenty-four  notes  in  the  octave.  13  it  fol- 
lowed a  suggestion  by  Euler  in  1739  that  each  manual  be  in  the 

11GiovanniBattistaDoni,  Compendiodel  trattato  de'  generi,  e  de'  modi  (Rome, 
1635),  Chap.  13. 

12Shohe'  Tanaka  (in  Vierteljahrsschrift  fur  Musikwissenschaft,  VI  [1890]  ,  85) 
was  in  error  in  showing  these  notes  of  Doni  as  only  a  comma  higher  than 
the  lower  note  of  the  pair  forming  the  semitone. 

13Helmholtz,  Sensations  of  Tone,  p.  316  f. 

Ill 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Pythagorean  tuning,  the  one  manual  a  comma  higher  than  the 
other.  General  Thompson  followed  Doni's  lead  by  having  three 
manuals  on  his  Enharmonic  Organ,  with  forty  different  pitches 
in  the  octave.  Henry  Poole's  Euharmonic  Organ  had  only  two 
black  keys  on  the  keyboard;  but  through  a  series  of  eleven  ped- 
als all  the  notes  could  be  transposed  into  five  sharp  and  five  flat 
keys,  giving  fifty  distinct  pitches  in  the  octave. 

Liston's  organ  also  relied  upon  pedals  to  obtain  a  great  vari- 
ety of  notes  with  the  minimum  number  of  keys.  14  With  only 
twelve  keys  to  the  octave,  tuned  in  just  intonation,  he  was  able 
by  means  of  six  pedals  to  add  their  enharmonic  equivalents,  thus 
having  twenty-four  notes  in  his  normal  scale.  These  are  shown 
in  Table  108.  Then  by  three  acute  pedals  all  these  notes  could 
be  raised  in  pitch  by  a  comma.  Two  grave  pedals  similarly 
lowered  nine  or  eleven  of  the  normal  notes  by  a  comma.  Thus 
Liston  had  a  total  of  fifty -nine  pitches  available. 

Of  Liston's  fifty-nine  notes,  there  were  ten  pairs,  such  as 
DId0-C*~2,  which  differed  by  the  schisma,  2  cents.  Further- 
more, Cx-3  and  E#~3  differed  by  only  six  cents  from  D*3  +1  and 
F""1"3  respectively,  and  could  be  considered  equivalent  pairs  also. 
Thus  there  were  essentially  only  47  separate  pitches.  These  in- 
cluded four  larger  intervals:  between  C  and  C*~2  and  between 
Cx"4  and  D"1  there  were  two  commas;  between  E#_1  and  F#~' 
and  between  A#~  and  B  there  were  three.  If  these  larger  in- 
tervals had  been  divided,  the  octave  would  have  contained  43  + 
2x2  +  2x3  =  53  commas,  which  is  the  number  one  might  have 


Table  108.    Liston's  Enharmonic  Organ 

B#-3        Fx-3  Cx-3 

G*-2        D*-2  A#-2  E*-2 

A-1  E-1  B"1  F#_1  C#_1 

Bb°  Fo  co  QO  Do 

^b-t-1     qD+i  Db+i  ^b+i  e^1 

nbb  +2  Tpb  +2 


14Henry  Liston,  An  Essay  upon  Perfect  Intonation  (Edinburgh,  1812),  pp.    3-7, 
33-40. 

112 


MULTIPLE  DIVISION 


anticipated.  These  "commas"  are  not  all  the  same  size.  The 
ditonic  comma  does  not  occur  at  all  except  as  the  sum  of  the 
syntonic  comma  and  the  schisma.  The  syntonic  comma  is,  as  is 
evident  from  the  scheme  of  pedals,  the  most  common  interval. 
But  intervals  of  20  cents,  as  D*~2  -E*30 ,  and  of  26  cents,  as 
G+1  -G*~3  ,  also  occur. 

More  ambitious  was  Steiner's  system.  °  For  the  key  of  C 
he  used  12  notes  in  just  intonation,  symmetrically  arranged  in 
three  groups  of  4  notes  each.  But  these  could  be  transposed  me- 
chanically into  any  of  12  different  keys,  the  keynotes  being  tuned 
by  perfect  fifths.  Thus  there  were  144  notes,  but  only  45  distinct 
pitches.  Shohe  Tanaka  adopted  Steiner's  idea  of  having  12  key- 
notes in  Pythagorean  tuning,  for  mechanical  transposition.  But 
he  extended  his  keyboard  to  26  different  notes,  as  shown  in  Ta- 
ble 10S.  Of  the  312  notes  to  the  octave  of  Tanaka's  "Transponir- 
Harmonium"  or  "Enharmonium,"  there  were  only  70  unduplicated 
pitches,  no  more  than  on  an  organ  described  by  Ellis  which  had 
a  total  of  14  x  11  or  154  notes  to  the  octave,  with  70  separate 
pitches. 

Table  109.    Tanaka's  Enharmonium 

F#-2  c#-2  G#-2  D*-2  A#-2  E#-2 

G"1  D"1  A-1  E-1  B"1  F#_1      C*"1 

Bb°  Fo  co  Go  Do  Ao  £o 

Qb+i  dd+1  Ab+1  Eb+1  Bb+1  F+1 

Equal  Divisions 

With  Tanaka's  Enharmonium  we  may  safely  drop  the  subject 
of  just  intonation  extended.  The  theory  is  simple  enough:  pro- 
vide at  least  four  sets  of  notes,  each  set  being  in  Pythagorean 
tuning  and  forming  just  major  thirds  with  the  notes  in  another 
set;  construct  a  keyboard  upon  which  these  notes  may  be  played 
with  the  minimum  of  inconvenience.  Only  in  the  design  of  the 
keyboards  did  the  inventors  show  their  ingenuity,  an  ingenuity 
that  might  better  have  been  devoted  to  something  more  practical. 

15Tanaka,  op.  cit.,  pp.  18  f.  and  23  ff. 

113 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


The  other  direction  in  which  multiple  division  developed  had 
far  greater  possibilities.  This  was  the  division  of  the  octave  into 
more  than  twelve  acoustically  equal  parts.  "  Any  regular  sys- 
tem of  tuning  —  a  system  constructed  on  a  fixed  value  of  the 
fifth  —  will  eventually  reach  a  point  where  its  "comma,"  the  er- 
ror for  the  enharmonic  equivalent  of  the  keynote,  is  small  enough 
to  be  disregarded.  Thus  we  have  closed  systems  that  agree 
more  or  less  closely  with  the  various  types  of  meantone  temper- 
ament, etc. 

If  the  Pythagorean  tuning  is  extended  to  17  notes,  an  interval 
of  66  cents  is  formed  — a  doubly  diminished  third,  as  Ax-C.  Di- 
vided among  17  notes,  the  deficit  is  about  4  cents,  the  amount  by 
which  each  fifth  must  be  raised  to  have  a  closed  system.  The 
fifth  (now  taken  as  10/17  octave)  contains  706  cents,  being  raised 
by  about  the  same  amount  that  it  is  lowered  in  the  Silbermann 
variety  of  meantone  temperament.  The  major  third  (6/17  octave) 
contains  423  cents,  being  more  than  twice  as  sharp  as  it  is  in 
equal  temperament,  and  the  minor  third  is  correspondingly  very 
flat.  If  we  take  5  parts  for  the  third,  this  becomes  a  neutral  third 
of  353  cents,  such  as  the  thirds  found  in  some  scales  of  the  Orient. 

In  the  17 -division,  the  tone  is  composed  of  3  equal  parts,  of 
which  the  diatonic  semitone  comprises  1  part  and  the  chromatic 
semitone  2  parts.  Since  the  diatonic  semitone,  70  cents,  is  even 
smaller  than  in  the  Pythagorean  tuning,  this  system  is  well 
adapted  to  melody.  It  is,  of  course,  wholly  unacceptable  for 
harmony  because  of  its  outsize  thirds.  It  is  notatedwith  5  sharps 
and  5  flats  only,  D*  and  A*  being  considered  the  equivalent  of 
Fb  and  Cb,  and  Gb  and  Db  the  equivalent  of  E#  and  B#»  The  17- 
division  is  the  well-known  Arabian  scale  of  third-tones. *-1 

A  much  more  popular  system  is  the  19-division.  It  arises  in 
much  the  same  way  as  the  17 -division,  except  that,  as  in  just  in - 

l"For  the  sake  of  completeness  two  smaller  divisions  should  be  mentioned: 
the  Javanese  equal  pentatonic  and  the  Siamese  equal  heptatonic.  For  a 
strange  reference  to  the  latter  see  J.  Murray  Barbour,  "Nierop's  Hacke- 
bort,"  Musical  Quarterly,  XX  (1934),  312-319. 

* 'Joseph  Sauveur  ("Systeme  general  des  intervalles  des  sons,"  M^moires  de 
l'academie  royale  des  sciences,  1701,  pp.  445  f.)  made  an  early  reference 
to  this  scale,  and  of  course  it  is  discussed  in  all  modern  accounts  of  Arabian 
theory. 

114 


MULTIPLE  DIVISION 


tonation,  the  diatonic  semitone  is  considered  the  larger,  with  2 
parts  to  1  for  the  chromatic  semitone.  Since  the  octave  contains 
5  tones  and  2  semitones,  it  will  have  5x3  +  2x2=  19  parts. 
The  history  of  the  19-division  goes  back  to  the  middle  of  the  six- 
teenth century,  whenZarlino  and  Salinas  discussed,  among  types 
of  meantone  tuning,  one  in  which  the  fifth  was  tempered  by  1/3 
comma.  Like  the  other  two  types  (1/4  and  2/7  comma)  it  was  in- 
tended for  a  cembalo  with  19  notes  to  the  octave.  °  Salinas' 
claim  as  inventor  has  not  been  disputed.  He  was  rather  apolo- 
getic concerning  it,  because  of  its  greater  deviation  from  pure 
intervals  than  the  other  two.  He  apparently  did  not  realize  that 
this  could  not  be  distinguished  from  an  equal  division  into  19 
parts,  and  that  thus,  as  a  closed  system,  it  possessed  a  great 
advantage.  It  can  be  notated  with  6  sharps  and  6  flats,  Cb  being 
the  equivalent  of  B*  and  E*  of  Fb. 

We  have  plenty  of  evidence  from  past  centuries  of  cembali 
with  19  notes  in  the  octave,  for  which  this  division  would  have 
been  the  ideal  tuning.  Zarlino19  described  such  a  cembalo  that 
Master  Domenico  Pesarese  had  made  for  him.  Elsasz  is  fre- 
quently but  erroneously  called  the  inventor  of  the  19-note  cem- 
balo, because  his  instrument  is  described  in  Praetorius'  Syn- 
tagma. 

After  having  been  neglected  during  the  nineteenth  century  for 
the  more  elaborate  systems  such  as  have  been  described  in  the 
previous  section  of  this  chapter,  the  19-division  was  revived  in 
the  second  quarter  of  the  twentieth  century.  It  has  had  eloquent 
contemporary  advocates  in  Ariel,  Kornerup,  and  Yasser.  Of  all 
these  enthusiasts,  Yasser  has  gone  to  the  greatest  pains  to  show 
the  construction  of  the  system  and  its  possibilities.  He  differs 
radically  from  its  other  adherents,  who  have  proposed  it  partly 
for  the  sake  of  differentiating  enharmonic  pairs  of  notes,  but 
chiefly  because  its  triads  are  more  consonant  than  those  of  equal 
temperament.    Yasser  holds  that  the  harmony  of  Scriabin  and  the 

*°See  Chapter  III  for  further  discussion  of  the  various  equivalents  of  the  cy- 
clic multiple  systems. 

^Institutioni  armoniche,  p.  140. 

^Joseph  Yasser,  A  Theory  of  Evolving  Tonality  (New  York,  1932). 

115 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


tone-rows  of  Schonberg  show  an  intuitive  striving  toward  the  19- 
division,  since  a  scale  as  used  should  contain  unequal  divisions, 
being  a  selection  from  an  equal  division  of  more  parts.  Thus 
the  Siamese  scale  of  7  equal  parts  is  suitable  for  pentatonic 
melodies;  the  ordinary  12-note  chromatic  scale,  for  heptatonic 
melodies;  and  the  19-division  for  melodies  built  upon  the  12- 
note  scale.  Yasser's  attempt  to  give  a  historical  foundation  is 
so  defective  that  his  case  emerges  considerably  weaker  than  if 
he  had  presented  his  system  simply  from  the  speculative  point 
of  view. 

There  does  not  seem  to  be  much  chance  of  the  19-division 
coming  into  use  in  our  day.  Its  thirds  and  fifths  have  been  dis- 
cussed in  Chapter  III.  To  modern  ears,  accustomed  to  the  sharp 
major  thirds  of  equal  temperament,  the  thirds  of  379  cents,  1/3 
comma  flat,  would  sound  insipid  in  the  extreme.  There  would 
seem  to  be  a  better  chance  for  the  acceptance  of  a  system  that 
does  not  differ  so  markedly  in  its  intervals  from  our  own. 

The  22 -division  belongs  next  in  our  study  of  equal  divisions. 
It  was  not  discussed  by  Sauveur,  Romieu,  or  Drobisch.  In  fact, 
Bosanquet  did  not  even  mention  it  in  his  comprehensive  book  on 
temperament,  although  Opelt  had  treated  it  carefully  twenty -five 
years  before.  *  But  the  following  year  Bosanquet  contributed 
an  article  to  the  Royal  Society,  "On  the  Hindoo  Division  of  the 
Octave."  In  it  he  referred  to  S.  M.  Tagore's  Hindu  Music  and 
an  article  in  Fetis'  Histoire  generate.  There  the  Hindoo  scale 
was  said  to  consist  of  22  small  intervals  called  "S'rutis."  If 
these  are  considered  equal,  a  new  system  arises  with  "practi- 
cally perfect"  major  thirds  (actually,  being  381.5  cents,  they  are 
almost  5  cents  flat)  and  very  sharp  fifths  (709  cents,  or  7  cents 
sharp).  Riemann  later  was  to  include  the  22-division  in  his  dis- 
cussion of  various  systems,  and  it  is  frequently  mentioned  today. 
Unfortunately,  the  Hindoo  theory  does  not  make  the  S'rutis  all 
equal,  but  that  does  not  prevent  the  division  from  finding  an  hon- 
ored place  among  these  others. 

The  thirds  of  the  22-division  are  better  than  those  of  the  19- 
division,  and  its  fifths  are  no  worse.    However,  it  is  not  so  good 


21F.  W.  Opelt,  Allgemeine  Theorie  der  Musik,  Chap.  IV. 
116 


MULTIPLE  DIVISION 


a  system  for  the  performance  of  European  music.  The  difficulty 
lies  in  the  formation  of  the  major  third.  The  fifth  is  taken  as 
13/22  octave,  whence  the  tone  has  4  parts  and  the  ditone,  8.  But 
8/22  octave  is  436  cents,  an  impossibly  high  value.  Hence  the 
major  third  must  be  only  7  parts,  or  381.5  cents.  This  means 
that  D#  is  taken  as  the  major  third  above  C,  and  Fb  (or  Cx)  as 
the  third  above  B.  This  is  an  awkward  feature,  but  one  that  we 
shall  run  into  with  most  of  these  equal  divisions.  It  is  not  or- 
dinarily possible  to  retain  our  ideas  of  tone  relations  while  mak- 
ing a  division  of  the  octave  that  will  provide  good  fifths  and  thirds. 

The  24 -division  has  the  same  good  fifths  and  sharp  thirds  as 
the  12-division,  and  the  deviations  for  the  29-division  are  very 
similar,  but  with  plus  and  minus  signs  reversed.  Both  the  25- 
and  the  28-divisions  have  good  thirds  and  quite  poor  fifths.  So 
none  of  these  four  divisions  is  of  great  import.  The  24 -division 
does  have  its  place,  as  a  possible  realization  of  Aristoxenus' 
theory  that  the  enharmonic  diesis  is  a  true  quarter  tone,  the  half 
of  the  equal  semitone.  Kircher22  presented  it  as  such,  together 
with  a  geometrical  method  of  obtaining  the  quarter  tones  on  the 
monochordo  Rossi2**  later  gave  the  string- lengths  for  equal 
quarter  tones,  and  Neidhardt  offered  a  similar  table  many  years 
afterwards. 24  The  29-division  has  its  place  as  a  member  in  the 
series  that  contains  the  17 -division,  but  that  fact  does  not  im- 
prove the  quality  of  its  thirds. 

The  next  system  of  importance  is  the  31-division.  It  is  the 
most  ancient  of  them  all  and  well  worth  the  attention  that  has 
been  given  to  it.  Observe  that  31  logically  follows  19  in  the  Fi- 
bonacci series:    5,  7,  12,  19,  31,  50,  81, This  system  was 

first  described  by  Vicentino25  in  1555,  as  the  method  of  tuning 
his  Archicembalo .  In  theory  this  was  constructed  in  an  attempt 
to  reconcile  the  ideas  of  the  ancient  Greeks  with  those  of  six- 
teenth century  practice.  In  reality  it  was  a  clever  method  for 
extending  the  usual  meantone  temperament  of  1/4  comma  until 


22Musurgia  universalis,  I,  p.  208. 

2^Sistema  musico,  p.  102. 

24J.  G.  Neidhardt,  Sectio  canonis  harmonici,  p.  31. 

25L'antica  musica,  Book  5,  Chaps.  3-5. 


117 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


it  formed  practically  a  closed  system. 

The  Archicembalo  contained  six  ranks  of  keys,  of  which  the 
first  two  represented  the  ordinary  harpsichord  keyboard  with  7 
natural  keys,  3  sharps,  and  2  flats.  The  third  "order"  contained 
4  more  sharps  and  3  flats.  The  fourth  order  continued  the  flat 
succession  with  7  more  keys,  and  the  fifth  added  5  more  sharps. 
(The  sixth  order  is  in  tune  with  the  first.)  Thus  all  the  notes 
would  lie  in  a  succession  of  fifths  from  G*30  to  Ax,  and  the  cir- 
cle would  be  completed  by  taking  Ex  as  equivalent  to  G^b  or  C^b 
to  Ax.  (Vicentino  himself  gave  a  second  tuning  to  the  fourth  or- 
der that  showed  that  he  considered  the  above  to  be  equivalent 
pitches.) 

Vicentino  specified  that  the  first  three  orders  of  the  Archi- 
cembalo should  be  tuned  "justly  with  the  temperament  of  the 
flattened  fifth,  according  to  the  usage  and  tuning  common  to  all 
the  keyboard  instruments,  as  organs,  cembali,  clavichords,  and 
the  like."  But  the  other  three  orders  may  be  tuned  "with  the 
perfect  fifth"  to  the  first  three  orders.  For  example,  the  G  of 
the  fourth  order  (that  is,  Abb)  is  to  be  a  perfect  fifth  above  the  C 
of  the  first  order.  It  must  be  admitted  that  this  part  of  Vicen- 
tino' s  scheme  does  not  seem  to  make  sense. 

If  we  ignore  this  puzzling  doctrine  of  the  perfect  fifth,  we 
have  a  logical  system,  formed  by  a  complete  sequence  of  31  tem- 
pered fifths.  The  amount  of  tempering  is  not  specified,  but  was 
to  be  the  same  as  that  of  common  practice.  The  common  prac- 
tice was  the  ordinary  meantone  temperament,  in  which  major 
thirds  are  perfect.    This  is  undoubtedly  what  Vicentino  used. 

By  logarithms  Christian  Huyghens^"  showed  that  the  31-di- 
vision  does  not  differ  perceptibly  from  the  1/4-comma  tempera- 
ment. More  specifically  he  said:  "The  fifth  of  our  division  is 
no  more  than  1/110  comma  higher  than  the  tempered  fifths,  which 
difference  is  entirely  imperceptible;  but  which  would  render  that 
consonance  so  much  the  more  perfect."  Riemann^'  was  con- 
fused by  this  remark,  not  realizing  that  Huyghens  meant  that  this 
fifth  was  1/110  comma  higher  than  a  fifth  tempered  by  1/4  comma. 

26"Novus  cyclus  harmonious,"  Opera  varia  (Leyden,  1724),  pp.  747-754. 


2'Geschichte  der  Musiktheorie,  p.  359. 
118 


MULTIPLE  DIVISION 


The  difference  between  the  logarithm  of  the  meantone  fifth, 
.174725011,  and  that  of  21B/3\  .1757916100,  is  .0000491089,  which 
is  quite  close  to  1/110  of  the  logarithm  of  the  syntonic  comma, 
.0053951317. 

Tanaka^S  and  Riemann  have  described  Gonzaga's  harpsichord 
intheMuseoCivico  inBologna,  dated  1606.  Essentially  the  same 
as  Vicentino's  instrument,  its  arrangement  of  notes  is  somewhat 
different,  the  second  row,  for  example,  consisting  solely  of 
sharped  notes,  instead  of  3  sharps  and  2  flats.  Father  Scipione 
Stella's  eight-manual  harpsichord  also  resembled  Vicentino's, 
but  had  a  couple  of  manuals  duplicated  to  facilitate  the  execu- 
tion.29 

An  improved  version  of  Vicentino's  Archicembalo  was  Colon - 
na's  6 -manual  Sambuca  Lincea.30  The  difficulty  with  Vicentino's 
system  was  the  unsystematic  arrangement  of  the  second  and 
third  orders.  Both  C#  and  Eb,  for  example,  were  in  the  second 
order,  while  Db  and  D*  were  in  the  third.  If  the  instrument  was 
to  be  considered  merely  an  extension  of  an  ordinary  cembalo 
with  twelve  notes  in  the  octave,  such  an  arrangement  was  no 
doubt  good  enough.  But,  for  its  complete  possibilities  to  be 
available,  any  such  instrument  needs  what  Bosanquet  called  a 
"generalized  keyboard." 

Colonna  came  close  to  supplying  this  lack.  Each  of  his  or- 
ders contained  seven  notes,  and  was  1/5  tone  above  the  preced- 
ing order.  In  our  notation,  the  notes  between  C  in  the  first  or- 
der and  D  in  the  sixth  would  be  DDb,  C*,  Db,  and  Cx„  Colonna' s 
notation  for  them  was  Cx,  C#,  DD,  and  C*,  respectively.  This  is 
very  clumsy;  but  his  idea  of  the  division  was  entirely  correct, 
as  can  be  seen  from  the  scales  he  listed  as  examples  of  the  ca- 
pabilities of  the  instrument.  He  included  such  remote  major 
keys  as  Cb,  A#,  Ebb,  and  G#  -  all  of  course  with  his  peculiar 
notation. 

28Shoh£  Tanaka,  in  Vierteljahrsschrift   fur   Musikwissenschaft,   VI   (1890), 
pp.  74  f.  ~ 

29Fabio  Colonna,  La  sambuca  lincea  (Naples,  1618),  p.  6. 
3Qlbid.,  passim. 


119 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


The  germ  of  the  31-division  lay  in  the  contention  of  Marchet- 
tus  of  Padua  that  a  tone  could  be  divided  into  five  parts.  After 
Vicentino,  Salinas  and  Mersenne  discussed  the  system  without 
realizing  its  value.  Hizler31  referred  to  a  31-note  octave,  but 
used  in  practice  only  13  notes,  having  both  a  D#  and  an  E". 
Rossi3^  anticipated  Huyghens  in  obtaining  by  logarithms  the 
string- lengths  for  the  31-division,  but  did  not  call  attention  to 
the  fact  that  its  pitches  were  so  close  to  those  of  the  meantone 
temperament  which  he  also  presented.  (With  A  at  41472,  his 
meantone  E  was  27734,  the  31-division  E,  27730.)  Gallimard33 
was  to  follow  Huyghen's  lead  in  comparing  the  logarithms  of  the 
two  temperaments.  Van  Blankenburg3"*  was  to  use  the  31-divi- 
sion as  a  sort  of  tuning  measure,  much  as  Sauveur  used  the  43- 
division  and  Mercator  the  53-division.  According  to  van  Blank  - 
enburg,  Neidhardt's  equal  temperament  was  full  of  "young  wolves, 
each  1/3  of  the  large  wolf,"  because  the  major  third  of  equal 
temperament  contains  10  1/3  parts  instead  of  the  10  parts  of  the 
31-division. 

The  string-lengths  for  the  31-division  were  also  given  by 
Ambrose  Warren,3^  for  the  octave  8000.0  to  4000.0.  Warren 
showed  how  this  temperament  could  be  applied  to  the  fingerboard 
of  the  violin,  for  a  string  13  inches  long. 

For  obtaining  the  31-division  mechanically,  Rossi  recom- 
mended the  mesolabium.  Salinas,  Zarlino,  and  Philander  have 
stated  that  the  mesolabium  could  be  used  for  finding  an  unlimited 
number  of  geometrical  means  between  two  lines,  provided  the 
number  of  parallelograms  was  increased  correspondingly.  Per- 
haps so,  but  Rossi3**  was  undoubtedly  correct  in  saying  that  "in 
dividing  the  octave  into  31  parts  you  will  experience  greater  dif- 

31Daniel  Hizler,  Extract  aus  der  neuen  Musica  Oder  Singkunst    (Niirnberg, 
1623),  p.  31. 

^^Sistema  musico,  pp.  86,  64. 

33J.  E.  Gallimard,   L'arithmetique  des  musiciens  (Paris,  1754),   Table  XVI, 
p.  25. 

3**Elementa  musica,  p.  115. 

35The  Tonometer  (London,  1725),  table  at  end  of  book. 

3"Sistema  musico,  p.  111. 

120 


MULTIPLE  DIVISION 


ficulty  because  of  the  great  number  of  rectangles,"  and  Mer- 
senne37  said  flatly  that  it  "is  of  no  use  except  for  finding  two 
means  between  two  given  lines." 

Romieu^°  included  the  31 -division  among  those  for  which  he 
had  obtained  correspondences,  calling  it  a  temperament  of  2/9 
comma.  This  is  not  very  close,  for  1/4  -  1/110  =  53/220.  (Dro- 
bisch's  74-division  is  the  real  2/9-comma  temperament.)  It  is 
possible  that  writers  before  Romieu  had  this  tuning  in  mind  when 
they  wrote  about  the  2/9-comma  temperament.  Printzy^  for  ex- 
ample, spoke  of  a  "still  earlier"  temperament  that  took  2/9 
comma  from  each  fifth.  Earlier,  perhaps,  than  Zarlino's  2/7 
comma,  which  he  had  been  discussing  previously.  But  Lemme 
Rossi,  who  gave  a  detailed  treatment  to  the  2/9-comma  tuning, 
did  not  identify  it  with  the  31 -division. 

The  34-division  is  a  positive  system,  like  the  22-division. 
That  is,  its  fifth  of  706  cents  is  larger  than  the  perfect  fifth,  be- 
ing the  same  size  as  for  the  17 -division.  Its  third  is  about  2 
cents  sharp.  Thus  it  provides  slightly  greater  consonance  than 
the  31 -division.  But,  like  the  22-division,  it  has  remained  one 
of  the  stepchildren  of  multiple  division,  largely  because  it  is  in 
a  series  for  which  ordinary  notation  cannot  be  used.  There  is  a 
surprising  mention  of  the  34-division  by  Cyriac  Schneegass  in 
1591  (see  Chapter  III),  but  his  own  monochord  came  closer  to 
the  2/9-comma  division.  Bosanquet  had  indicated  the  relation 
between  the  22-  and  34-divisions,  and  had  praised  the  56-  and 
87-divisions  also  as  similar  systems.  Opelt,  too,  has  included 
it  in  his  fairly  short  list. 

The  36 -division  has  little  to  recommend  it,  although  its  string- 
lengths  were  worked  out  by  Berlin,'*"  and  Appun  and  Oettingen 
both  found  it  worth  describing.'*-'- 

The  41-division  has  excellent  fifths  (702.4  cents),  but  thirds 

3'Harmonie  universelle,  p.  224. 

JOIn  Memoires  de  1  academie  royale  des  sciences,  1758,  p.  837. 

^"phrynis  Mytilenaeus  oder  der  satyrische  Componist,  p.  88. 

40Johann  Daniel  Berlin,  Anleitung  zur  Tonometrie  (Copenhagen  and  Leipzig, 
1767),  pp.  26-27. 

41Hugo  Riemann,  Populare  Darstellung  der  Akustik  (Berlin,  1896),  p.  138. 

121 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


(380.5)  that  are  almost  six  cents  flat,  being  in  this  latter  respect 
inferior  to  the  31-  and  34 -divisions.  It  occurs  in  a  worthy  se- 
ries: 12,  17,  29,  41,  53, ... .  This  system  was  not  singled  out 
by  any  of  the  earlier  writers,  but  received  considerable  atten- 
tion from  such  nineteenth  century  theorists  as  Delezenne,  Dro- 
bisch,  andBosanquet.  Paul  von  Janko"*^  set  himself  the  task  of  as- 
certaining the  best  system  between  12  and  53  divisions,  and  chose 
the  41 -division.  Rather  naively,  he  concluded  he  had  discovered 
this  system,  since  Riemann  had  not  mentioned  it! 

The  43-division  is  associated  with  the  name  of  Sauveur, 4^ 
who  used  its  intervals  (Merides)  as  a  unit  of  musical  measure. 
The  Merides  were  divided  into  seven  parts  called  Eptamerides. 
For  more  subtle  distinctions,  Sauveur  suggested  using  Decam- 
erides,  10  of  which  comprised  one  Eptameride.  But  he  did  not 
use  the  Decamerides  in  practice.  Thus  there  were  43  x  7  =  301 
Eptamerides  in  the  octave,  or  3010  Decamerides.  Since  .30103 
is  the  common  logarithm  of  2,  it  is  possible  to  convert  directly 
from  logarithms  to  Eptamerides  by  dropping  the  decimal  point 
and  all  but  the  first  three  digits  of  the  logarithm. 

The  43-division  is  a  closed  system  approximating  the  1/5- 
comma  variety  of  meantone  temperament,  which,  as  we  saw  in 
Chapter  III,  had  been  mentioned  by  Verheijen  and  Rossi.  Its 
thirds  and  fifths  have  an  equal  and  opposite  error  of  slightly  over 
four  cents,  thus  making  it  somewhat  inferior  to  the  34 -division, 
although  the  equality  of  the  error  may  have  some  weight  in  rank- 
ing the  two  systems.  Since  43  is  a  number  occurring  in  a  useful 
series  for  multiple  division  —  12,  19,  31,  43,  55, ...  —  this  divi- 
sion was  treated  by  Romieu,  Opelt,  Drobisch,  and  Bosanquet. 

The  50-division  need  not  detain  us  long.  It  may  be  thought  of 
as  an  octave  composed  of  ditonic  commas,  since  1200  r  24  =  50. 
It  was  advocated  by  Henfling  in  1710  and  criticized  by  Sauveur44 
the  following   year.    A  century  later  Opelt  was  to  mention  it. 

42"Uber  mehr  als  zwolfstufige  gleichschwebende  Temperaturen,"    Beitrage 
zur  Akustik  und  Musikwissenschaft,  1901,  pp.  6-12. 

43joseph  Sauveur,  in  Memoires  de  l'academie  royale  des  sciences,  1701,  pp. 
403-498. 

44joseph  Sauveur,  "Table   generate  des    systemes    temp£re's  de    musique," 
Mlmoires  de  l'academie  royale  des  sciences,  1711,  p.  406  f. 

122 


MULTIPLE  DIVISION 


Bosanquet  has  included  it  as  a  member  of  the  series:  12,  19, 
31,  50, ....  This  division  shows  no  improvement  over  the  31- 
division.  Its  fifths  have  about  the  same  value  as  those  of  the 
latter,  and  its  thirds  are  flatter  than  the  latter' s  were  sharp. 
Kornerup^  has  waxed  lyrical  in  its  praise,  as  a  closed  system 
corresponding  to  Zarlino's  2/7 -comma  meantone  temperament. 
He  showed  that  the  value  for  Zarlino's  chromatic  semitone 
(70.6724  cents)  came  very  close  to  the  mean  of  the  chromatic 
semitones  for  the  19-  and  31 -divisions  (70.2886),  and  might  have 
added  that  this  similarity  extends  throughout,  since  all  three  are 
regular  systems.  He  found  that  the  greatest  deviation  of  the 
2/7  -comma  tuning  from  the  50-division  is  a  little  over  three  cents, 
and  is  much  less  for  most  notes.  We  shall  have  more  to  say 
later  about  the  special  part  of  Kornerup's  theory  that  has  caused 
him  to  overvalue  this  system. 

The  most  important  system  after  the  31-  is  the  53-division. 
In  theory  it  is  also  the  most  ancient.  According  to  Boethius,  " 
Pythagoras'  disciple  Philolaus  held  that,  since  the  tone  is  divis- 
ible into  minor  semitones  and  a  comma,  and  since  the  semitone 
is  divisible  into  two  diaschismata,  the  tone  is  then  divisible  into 
four  diaschismata  plus  a  comma.  If,  now,  the  diaschisma  is  taken 
as  two  commas  exactly,  the  tone  is  divided  into  nine  commas. 
(Note  what  was  said  about  the  ditonic  comma  in  connection  with 
the  50-division.) 

This  dictum  about  the  number  of  commas  in  a  tone  was  one 
of  the  most  persistent  parts  of  the  Pythagorean  system.  Writers 
in  the  early  sixteenth  century  sometimes  mentioned  the  fact  that 
there  are  nine  commas  in  a  tone,  without  giving  any  other  tuning 
lore.  They  probably  included,  however,  the  statement  that  the 
diatonic  semitone  contains  four  commas,  the  chromatic  semitone, 
five.  Amusingly  enough,  after  just  intonation  became  the  ideal, 
writers  continued  to  talk  about  commas;  but  now  it  was  the  chro- 
matic semitone  that  contained  four  commas,  the  diatonic  semi- 
tone, five. 

Since  the  Pythagorean  diatonic  semitone  contains  90  cents, 

45Thorvald   Kornerup,  Das  Tonsystem  des  Italieners  Zarlino  (Copenhagen, 
1930). 

46A.  M.  S.  Boethius,  De  institutione  musica,  Book  3,  Chap.  8. 

123 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


and  the  chromatic,  114,  their  ratio  is  3  3/4:4  3/4,  or  approxi- 
mately 4:5.  Similarly,  if  we  choose  the  larger  just  chromatic 
semitone  of  92  cents  and  the  smaller  just  diatonic  semitone  of 
112  cents,  the  ratio  will  be  4  1/2:5  1/2,  or,  again,  4:5.  But  the 
ratio  might  be  taken  as  5:6,  giving  rise  to  the  67-division  dis- 
cussed below.  The  comma,  taken  as  1/9  Pythagorean  tone,  would 
have  a  mean  value  of  22.7  cents,  lying  between  the  syntonic  and 
the  ditonic  commas. 

If  there  are  9  commas  in  a  tone,  the  octave  contains  5x9  + 
2  x  4  =  53  commas  —  provided  we  are  thinking  in  terms  of  the 
Pythagorean  tuning.  If  we  are  thinking  in  terms  of  just  intona- 
tion, with  a  large  diatonic  semitone,  there  will  be  5x9  +  2x5  = 
55  commas.    Thus  the  55-division  has  received  attention  also. 

There  are  several  advantages  to  the  53-division.  Its  fifths 
are  practically  perfect  (.1  cent  flat),  so  that  it  is  unnecessary  to 
use  a  monochord  for  tuning.  Its  thirds  are  very  slightly  flat 
(1.4  cents).  However,  since  it  is  a  positive  system,  with  fifths 
sharper  than  those  of  equal  temperament,  the  pure  major  third 
above  C  is  F",  with  17  parts,  whereas  C-E  represents  the  Pyth- 
agorean third,  with  18  parts.  This  would  be  confusing  to  the 
performer. 

After  the  time  of  the  Greeks,  the  history  of  the  53-division 
takes  us  to  China,  where  the  Pythagorean  tuning  had  been  known 
for  many  centuries,  probably  since  the  invasion  of  Alexander  the 
Great.  In  1713  it  was  confirmed  as  the  official  scale,  however 
widely  instrumental  tunings  may  have  differed  from  it  in  practice. 

One  of  the  most  remarkable  of  the  early  Chinese  theorists 
was  King  FSng,  who,  according  to  Courant,  "calculated  ex- 
actly the  proportional  numbers  to  60  111,"  that  is,  he  extended  the 
Pythagorean  system  to  60  notes.  These  results  were  published 
by  Seu-ma  Pyeou,  who  died  in  306  A.  D.  King  FSng  observed 
that  the  54th  note  was  almost  identical  with  the  first  note.  Cou- 
rant's  figures  are  177,147  for  the  first;   176,777  for  the  54th. 

Seventeenth  century  European  theorists  who  referred  defin- 
itely  to  this    system  include  Mersenne   and   Kircher.    Tanaka 


^Maurice  Courant,  "Chine  et  Core"e,"   Encyclopedic  de  la  musique  et  dic- 
tionnaire  du  conservatoire  (Paris,  1913),  Part  I,  Vol.  I,  p.  88. 

124 


MULTIPLE  DIVISION 


mentioned  Kircher's  name  in  this  connection,  thus  differing  from 
the  majority  of  his  contemporaries,  who  ascribed  the  system  to 
Mercator.  According  to  Holder,48  Nicholas  Mercator  had  "de- 
duced an  ingenious  Invention  of  finding  and  applying  a  least  Com- 
mon Measure  to  all  Harmonic  Intervals,  not  precisely  perfect, 
but  very  near  it."  This  was  the  division  into  53  commas.  There 
is  no  evidence,  in  Holder's  account,  that  Mercator  intended  this 
system  to  be  used  on  an  instrument.  It  was  to  be  merely  a 
"Common  Measure." 

Of  25  systems  that  Sauveur  discussed,  only  two,  the  17-  and 
53-divisions,  were  positive.  He  was  unable  to  appreciate  the 
splendid  value  of  the  thirds  of  the  latter,  since,  according  to  his 
theory,  its  thirds  would  have  to  be  as  large  as  Pythagorean  thirds. 
Romieu  did  not  even  mention  this  system.  Drobisch,  too,  did  not 
at  first  (1853)  appreciate  the  53-division,  discarding  it  because 
of  its  sharp  thirds.  But  two  years  later  he  re-evaluated  both  the 
41-  and  the  53-divisions,  showing  that  a  just  major  scale  could 
be  obtained  with  them  by  using  C  D  Fb  G  Bbb  Cb  C.49 

The  stage  was  thus  set  for  Bosanquet' s  detailed  study  of  mul- 
tiple division,  which  culminated  in  his  invention  of  the  "gener- 
alized keyboard"  for  regular  systems.  In  his  article  in  the  Royal 
Society's  Proceedings,  1874-75,  Bosanquet  gave  a  clear  and  com- 
prehensive treatment  of  regular  systems,  both  positive  and  neg- 
ative, with  a  possible  notation  for  them.  He  showed  how  various 
systems  could  be  applied  to  his  keyboard,  especially  the  53-  and 
118 -divisions.  In  his  symmetrical  arrangement,  84  keys  were 
needed  for  the  53  different  notes  in  the  octave.  Obviously,  then, 
Bosanquet' s  name  should  be  singled  out  for  especial  mention, 
since  he  applied  the  system  to  an  enharmonic  harmonium  and  did 
not  simply  discuss  it  as  his  predecessors  had  done. 

As  has  been  noted  above,  the  55-division  is  the  negative  coun- 
terpart of  the  53-division,  thus  having  the  advantage  that  ordi- 
nary notation  can  be  used.  That  is  its  only  advantage,  for  its 
fifths  (698.2  cents)  are  no  better  than  those  of  the  43-division, 

48william  Holder,  Treatise  .  .  .of  Harmony,  p.  79. 

49m,  W.  Drobisch,  "Uber  musikalische  Tonbestimmung  und  Temperatur," 
Abhandlungen  der  mathematisch-physischen  Classe  der  koniglich  s'achsis- 
chen  Gesellschaft  der  Wissenschaften,  IV  (1855),  82-86. 

125 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


and  its  thirds  (392.7  cents)  are  inferior  to  the  latter' s.  Sauveur 
devoted  considerable  space  to  this  system,  saying  it  was  "fol- 
lowed by  the  musicians."  This  is  a  reasonable  statement,  for 
this  system  corresponds  closely  to  the  1/6 -comma  variety  of 
meantone  temperament  favored  by  Silbermann.  Thus  we  have 
confirmation  from  France  of  the  spread  of  this  method. 

Romieu  showed  the  correspondence  between  the  55-division 
and  the  1/6-comma  tuning,  and  adopted  the  latter  for  his  "tem- 
perament anacratique.  "  He  referred  to  Sauveur,  and  also  to 
Ramarin's  system  as  given  in  Kircher.  Mattheson^l  presented 
this  division  from  Johann  Beer's  Schola  phonologica,  saying  that 
it  required  "that  an  octave  should  have  55  commas,  but  no  ma- 
jor or  minor  tones." 

Sorge,  after  disapproving  of  the  ordinary  1/4 -comma  mean- 
tone,  continued:  "I  am  better  pleased  by  the  famous  Capell- 
meister  Telemann's  system  of  intervals,  in  which  the  octave  is 
divided  into  55  geometrical  parts  (commas),  that  grow  smaller 
from  step  to  step."  52  Sorge  explained  that  in  its  complete  state 
it  could  not  be  used  on  the  clavier;  but  it  might  be  applied  to  the 
violin  and  to  certain  wind  instruments,  and  was  easiest  for  singers. 


^Correspondences  between  multiple  divisions  and  temperaments  by  fractional 
parts  of  the  syntonic  comma  can  be  worked  out  by  continued  fractions. 
When  the  temperament  of  the  fifth  is  1/2  comma,  the  octave  contains  26 
parts.  If  d  is  the  denominator  of  the  fractional  part  of  the  comma  (21.5 
cents),  the  following  formula  gives  the  parts  in  the  octave  for  2<d<:ll: 
Sd  =  7  +  12   |~114d  -  150.51  '  wnere  the  expression  in  brackets  is  to  betaken 

[l 2(2 1.5  -  2d)J 
to  the  nearest  integer.  The  list  of  correspondences  is:  1/3  comma,  19 
parts;  1/4  comma,  31  parts;  1/5  comma,  43  parts;  1/6  comma,  55  parts; 
1/7  comma,  91  parts;  1/8  comma,  139  parts;  1/9  comma,  247  parts;  1/10 
comma,  499  parts.  Temperaments  in  which  the  numerator  of  the  fraction 
is  2  are  formed  as  follows:    1+1  _  2  comma,  26  +  19  =  45  parts;    1  +  1  _  2 

2T~3  "  5  3  +  4  "  7 

comma,  19  +  31  =  50  parts;   1+1  _  2  comma,  31  +  43  =  74  parts;   1  +  1  _  2 

4  +  5  ~  9  5  +  6  "11 

comma,  43  +  55  =  98  parts,  etc. 

51J.  Mattheson,  Critica  musica  (Hamburg,  1722-25),  II,  73 f. 

"^Georg  Andreas  Sorge,  Gesprach  zwischen  einem  Musico  theoretico  und 
einem  Studioso  musices,  pp.  51  f. 

126 


MULTIPLE  DIVISION 


William  Jackson*^  found  that  the  octave  consists  of  55  10/12 
syntonic  commas,  or  670  units  of  1/12  comma.  He  might  well 
have  assumed  the  octave  to  contain  56  commas  precisely,  since 
this  is  a  fairly  good  division.  A  half  century  after  Jackson,  an 
anonymous  work  printed  in  Holland*^  stated  that  the  ratio  81:80 
is  contained  56  times  in  the  octave,  but  did  not  advocate  this  as 
a  system  of  multiple  division.  Bosanquet  mentioned  the  56 -divi- 
sion. It  has  excellent  thirds,  being  1  cent  flat,  as  in  the  28-di- 
vision.    Its  fifths  are  5  cents  sharp. 

The  58 -division  is  also  positive,  its  fifths  being  2  cents  sharp, 
as  in  the  29 -division,  and  its  thirds  being  7  cents  sharp.  This 
is  the  division  that  is  at  the  base  of  Dom  Bedos'  temperament,  ° 
although  he  chose  the  pitches  for  his  monochord  somewhat  ir- 
regularly from  it. 

There  are  only  a  few  other  systems  that  should  be  mentioned. 
The  65-division  has  splendid  fifths  (.5  cent  flat)  and  slightly 
sharp  thirds  (1.4  cents  sharp).  The  84-division,  on  the  other 
hand,  has  only  average  fifths  (2  cents  flat),  but  excellent  thirds 
(.6  cent  flat).  The  87-division  has  slightly  sharp  fifths  (1.4  cents 
sharp),  and  practically  perfect  thirds  (.1  cent  flat).  The  118-di- 
vision  has  both  fifths  and  thirds  that  are  superlative  (.5  cent  flat 
and  o2  sharp  respectively). 

The  above  four  systems  excel  all  others  with  more  than  53 
parts  in  the  octave.  But  the  specialists  in  multiple  division  have 
not  always  appreciated  them.  Sauveur,  for  example,  discussed 
the  67-,  74-,  98-,  105-,  112-,and  117 -divisions,  as  well  as  others 
that  are  no  better  than  they,  but  did  not  mention  any  of  the  four 
systems  in  the  previous  paragraph.  Romieu  did  not  discuss  any 
systems  beyond  the  55 -division,  but  would  have  approved  the  67-, 
79-,  and  91-divisions.  Drobisch  particularly  favored  the  74-di- 
vision  among  systems  that  formed  the  major  third  regularly,  as 


53A  Scheme  Demonstrating  the  Perfection  and  Harmony  of  Sounds   (London, 
1726  [?]),  chart. 

^Exposition  de  quelques  nouvelles  vues  mathematiques  dans  la  theorie  de  la 
musique  (Amsterdam,  1760),  p.  28. 

55Dom  Francois  Bedos  de  Celles,  L'art  du  facteur  d'orgues,  2nd  Part  (Paris, 
1770);  facsimile  ed.  (Kassel,  1935),  p.  430. 

127 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


C-E;  among  those  that  used  C-F13  as  a  major  third,  he  mentioned 
the  65-,  70-,  77-,  89-,  and  94-divisions,  and  found  the  53-  and 
118-divisions  best  of  all.  Bosanquet,  praising  most  highly  the 
53-  and  118-divisions,  had  kind  words  for  the  56-,  65-,  and  87- 
divisions  also. 

Theory  of  Multiple  Division 

The  reason  for  the  divergent  results  obtained  by  these  theo- 
rists is  that  each  had  a  different  theory  regarding  acceptable  di- 
visions of  the  octave.  Sauveur,  although  he  did  list  two  positive 
systems,  had  no  real  understanding  of  divisions  in  which  C-F*3 
could  be  a  major  third„  To  him,  the  diatonic  semitone  was  the 
larger:  the  problem  of  temperament  was  to  decide  upon  a  def- 
inite ratio  between  the  diatonic  and  chromatic  semitones,  and 
that  would  automatically  give  a  particular  division  of  the  octave. 
If,  for  example,  the  ratio  is  3:2,  there  are  5x7  +  2x4  =  43 
parts;  if  5:4,  there  are  5x9  +  2x5  =  55  parts.  We  have  pointed 
out  above  that  only  the  first  of  these  divisions  is  at  all  satisfac- 
tory. Let  us  see  what  the  limit  of  the  value  of  the  fifth  would  be 
if  the  (n  +  l):n  series  were  extended  indefinitely.  The  fifth  is 
(7n  +  4)/(12n  +  7)  octave,  and  its  limit,  as  n 5»  ©<=>  ,  is  7/12  oc- 
tave; that  is,  the  fifth  of  equal  temperament.  The  third,  similarly, 
approaches  1/3  octave.  Therefore,  the  farther  the  series  goes, 
the  better  become  its  fifths,  the  poorer  its  thirds.  This  would 
seem,  then,  to  be  an  inferior  theory. 

In  other  divisions  listed  by  Sauveur  the  difference  between 
the  two  sizes  of  semitone  was  two,  three,  or  even  four  parts. 
Here,  again,  the  fifth  eventually  comes  close  to  7/12  octave  and 
the  third  to  3/12  octave.  Romieu  followed  Sauveur' s  theory.  To 
an  extent  so  did  Bosanquet.  But  the  latter  added  the  theory  of 
positive  systems.    The  primary  positive  system  is  17,  29,  41, 

53,  65,  77,  89, Here  the  fifth  can  be  expressed  as  (7n  +  3)/ 

(12n  +  5)  octave.  Just  as  in  the  negative  systems  above,  the  limit 
of  this  ratio  is  7/12  octave,,  For  the  53-  and  65-divisions  the 
fifths  are  practically  perfect;  the  thirds  of  these  divisions  have 
approximately  equal,  but  opposite,  deviations.    This  suggests  a 


128 


MULTIPLE  DIVISION 


secondary  positive  system,  the  mean  between  the  former  two: 
118,  with  both  fifths  and  thirds  well-nigh  perfect.  But  there  is 
nothing  in  these  series  themselves  to  facilitate  choosing  the  best 
division  or  the  two  best.  That  had  to  be  ascertained  by  compar- 
ing the  intervals  in  the  various  divisions  after  they  had  been 
chosen.  Again  it  would  seem  as  if  there  were  an  arbitrary  fac- 
tor present. 

We  have  already  spoken  of  Kornerup  and  his  fondness  for  the 
50-division.56  His  "golden"  system  of  music  was  suggested  by 
a  study  made  by  P.S.  Wedell  and  N.  P.  J.  Bertelsen  in  1915.  By 
the  method  of  least  squares  they  obtained  the  following  octave 
series  in  which  both  the  major  third  (5:4)  and  the  augmented 
sixth  (that  is,  the  minor  seventh,  7:4)  approach  their  pure  val- 
ues:   3,   5,  7,   12,   19,   31,   50,  81,   131,  212,   343, These  of 

course  are  "golden"  numbers,  the  law  of  the  series  being  Sn  = 
sn-i  +  sn-2-   As  n ><*>,  s      >a[5-1      =.61803398,  a 

s~n  2 

an+  i 
ratio  which  Kornerup  called  go  .  It  is  this  ratio  which  is  used  in 
the  golden  section  of  a  line,  where  (1  -  co  )/co  =  6U,  and  which 
Kornerup  used  as  the  basis  of  his  tuning  system.  By  rather 
simple  arithmetic  we  find  that  the  golden  fifth  is  (15  -  /\[5)/22  oc- 
tave, or  696.2144738  cents.  The  golden  third  is  384.8579  cents, 
only  a  fair  approximation,  since  the  pure  third  is  386.3137  cents. 
Therefore,  even  if  the  series  is  continued  indefinitely,  the  fifth 
will  never  be  less  than  about  6  cents,  nor  the  third  than  1.5  cents 
flat.  Since  we  have  already  observed  several  systems  with  bet- 
ter thirds  and  fifths  than  this,  it  would  seem  as  if  the  golden 
system  is  an  ignis  fatuus. 

Drobisch57  gave  an  interesting  formula  which  combined  Bo- 
sanquet's  primary  and  secondary  positive  systems.  The  fifth  of 
these  systems  will  be:  (7n  -  l)/2(6n  -  1).  For  odd  values  of  n, 
the  octave  contains  6n  -  1  parts;  for  even  values,  twice  as  many. 


"'"See  Thorvald  Kornerup,  Das  goldene  Tonsystem  als  Fundament  des  theo- 
retischen  Akustik  (Copenhagen,  1935). 

"* 'In  Abhandlungen  der  mathematisch-physischen  Classe  der  koniglich  sach- 
sischen  Gesellschaft  der  Wissenschaften,  IV  (1855),  79  f. 

129 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Hence  he  obtained  the  series  (with  n  ranging  from  4  through  15)  : 
46,  29,  70,  41,  94,  53,  118,  65,  142,  77,  166,  89. 

Somewhat  more  general  was  Drobisch's  attempt  to  find  a  di- 
vision of  the  octave  that  would  insure  a  good  value  for  the  fifth. 
He  expressed  the  ratio  of  the  fifth  (log  3/2)  to  the  octave  (log  2) 
as  a  decimal,  .5849625,  or  as  a  fraction,  46797/80000.  From 
this  ratio,  by  binary  continued  fractions,  he  obtained  the  series 

2,  5,  12,  41,  53,  306,  665,    [15601]  , Next  he  found  all   the 

powers  of  3/2  from  the  13th  to  the  53rd,  in  order  to  ascertain 
which  approach  a  pure  octave.  This  should  have  checked  closely 
with  his  previous  list,  to  which  17  and  29  would  be  semi-conver- 
gents.  This,  however,  is  his  complete  list:  17,  19,  22,  29,  31, 
41,  43,  46,  51,  53.  Having  eliminated  all  positive  divisions  (those 
with  raised  fifths),  he  still  had  19,  31,  and  43  to  add  to  his  prev- 
ious list. 

Although  the  50-division  did  not  appear  on  either  list,  Dro- 
bisch  anticipated  Kornerup  by  showing  that  its  fifth  lies  almost 
exactly  between  the  fifths  of  the  19-  and  31 -divisions.  After 
these  promising  beginnings,  he  went  off  at  a  tangent  by  trying  to 
find,  by  least  squares,  the  value  of  the  fifth  that  would  produce 
the  best  values  for  five  different  intervals.  Then,  again  using 
continued  fractions,  he  found  that  successive  approximations  to 

this  value  (.5810541)  form  the  series:    2,  5,  7,  12,  31,  74, 

This  is  why  the  74-division  had  an  especial  appeal  for  him. 

Drobisch's  continued  fractions  were  the  first  really  scien- 
tific method  of  dividing  the  octave  with  regard  to  the  principal 
consonances,  the  thirds  and  the  fifths.  The  difficulty  with  it  is 
that  there  are  three  magnitudes  to  be  compared  (third,  fifth,  and 
octave),  but  only  one  ratio  (third  to  octave,  fifth  to  octave,  pos- 
sibly third  to  fifth)  can  be  approximated  by  binary  continued  frac- 
tions. If  we  must  choose  a  single  ratio,  it  is  better  to  use  that 
of  the  fifth  to  the  octave,  as  Drobisch  did,  since  the  third  may  be 
expressed  in  terms  of  the  fifth.  But  the  usual  formula,  T  =  4F  - 
20,  is  valid  only  through  O  =  12.  We  have  already  noted  that  as 
fine  a  musical  theorist  as  Sauveur  failed  to  appreciate  the  53- 
division,  since  he  used  the  above  formula  and  obtained  a  third 
that  was  one  part  large.    Since  the  syntonic  comma  is  about  1/56 


130 


MULTIPLE  DIVISION 


octave,  this  formula  will  fail  to  give  a  correct  number  of  parts 
for  the  third  for  any  octave  division  greater  than  28.  Thus  if 
O  =  41,  and  F  =  24,  the  formula  makes  T  =  4  x  24  -  2  x  41  =  14, 
whereas  the  correct  value  is  13.  If  O  =  665,  and  F  =  389,  T  = 
4  x  389  -  2  x  665  =  226,  instead  of  214.  Knowing  the  value  of  the 
comma,  we  can  correct  our  formula  to  read:    T  =  4F  -  20  -  TO"]. 

[_56j 
But  even  this  would  only  by  accident  give  a  value  for  the  third 
with  as  small  a  deviation  as  that  for  the  fifth  in  the  same  divi- 
sion.,   What  is  needed  is  a  method  that  will  approach  the  just  val- 
ues for  third  and  fifth  simultaneously. 

The  desired  solution  can  be  obtained  only  by  ternary  con- 
tinued fractions,  which  are  a  means  by  which  the  ratios  of  three 
numbers  may  be  approximated  simultaneously,  just  as  the  ra- 
tios of  two  numbers  may  be  approximated  by  binary  continued 
fractions.  When  the  ordinary  or  Jacobi  ternary  continued  frac- 
tions are  applied  to  the  logarithms  of  the  major  third  (5:4),  per- 
fect fifth  (3:2),  and  octave  (2:1),  the  octave  divisions  will  be: 
3,  25,  28,   31,  87,  817, 

There  are  two  serious  faults  in  these  results.  In  the  first 
place,  the  expansion  converges  too  rapidly,  and  we  are  interested 
chiefly  in  small  values,  those  for  which  the  octave  has  fewer  than 
100  parts.  In  the  second  place,  the  first  few  terms  are  foreign 
to  every  other  proposed  solution,  such  as  those  by  Sauveur  and 
Drobisch  on  previous  pages. 

To  insure  slow  convergence,  a  mixed  expansion  was  evolved, 
which  yields  the  following  excellent  series  of  octave  divisions: 

3,   5,  7,   12,   19,   31,   34,   53,  87,   118,   559,  612, 58  The  only 

serious  omission  is  the  Hindoo  division,  with  22  parts  in  the  oc- 
tave o  The  last  term  shown  above  (612)  was  said  by  Bosanquet  to 
have  been  considered  very  good  by  Captain  J.  Herschel. 

There  is  no  record  that  Captain  Herschel  ever  constructed 
an  experimental  instrument  with  612  separate  pitches  in  the  oc- 
tave. Even  if  he  had  done  so,  it  would  have  been  a  mechanical 
monster,  incapable  of  producing  genuine  music  at  the  hands  of  a 


^8J.  Murray  Barbour,  "Music  and  Ternary  Continued  Fractions,"  American 
Mathematical  Monthly,  LV  (1948),  545-555. 

131 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


performer.  With  the  possible  exception  of  the  19-  and  22-divi- 
sions,  the  same  can  be  said  of  all  these  attempts  at  multiple  di- 
vision. Bosanquet's  53-division  apparently  was  a  success  on  the 
harmonium  he  constructed  with  the  "generalized  keyboard."  But 
it,  too,  was  cumbersome  to  play,  and  would  have  been  very  ex- 
pensive if  applied  to  a  pipe  organ  or  piano.  Thus  the  mathemat- 
ical theory,  worked  out  laboriously  by  ternary  continued  frac- 
tions, remains  theory  and  nothing  more.  The  practice  for  the 
past  five  hundred  years  has  favored  almost  exclusively  systems 
with  only  twelve  different  pitches  in  the  octave.  There  seems  no 
immediate  prospect  of  that  practice  being  discarded  in  favor  of 
any  system  of  multiple  division. 


132 


Chapter  VII.    IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS1 


If  we  accept  Bosanquet's  definition  that  a  "regular"  tuning  sys- 
tem is  one  in  which  every  fifth,  or  every  fifth  save  one,  has  the 
same  value,  this  would  include  the  Pythagorean  tuning,  equal  tem- 
perament, and  the  several  varieties  of  the  meantone  tempera- 
ment, as  well  as  equal  divisions  with  more  than  twelve  notes  in 
the  octave.  With  the  addition  of  just  intonation,  it  would  seem  as 
if  this  covered  the  ground  pretty  thoroughly.  There  are,  however, 
a  great  many  tuning  systems  that  do  not  fall  into  any  of  the  above- 
mentioned  classes.  At  first  glance  these  irregular  systems  pre- 
sent a  bewildering  variety.  But  some  of  them  have  been  offered 
by  their  sponsors  as  modifications  of  existing  tuning  systems, 
and  others,  although  not  so  designated,  are  also  closely  related 
to  regular  systems.  In  fact,  it  is  possible,  by  making  the  bounds 
sufficiently  elastic,  to  fit  every  one  of  these  irregular  systems 
into  one  or  another  of  certain  subclasses.  So  that,  unless  we  re- 
tain Bosanquet's  strict  definition,  there  is  no  such  thing  as  an 
irregular  system  —  one  that  is  wholly  a  law  unto  itself! 

Our  first  group  of  irregular  temperaments  consists  of  modi- 
fications of  the  meantone  temperament.  The  meantone  wolf  fifth 
is  35  cents  sharp.  The  simplest  modification  of  this  tempera- 
ment is  to  divide  this  excess  equally  between  the  fifths  C*-G^ 
and  G*(Ab)-Eb  (see  Table  110).    This  is  the  modification  gener- 

Table  110.    Meantone  Temperament  with  Two  Sharp  Fifths 

Names     C    C#    D        Eb       E        F       F#       G       G#      A         Bb         B  C 

Cents      0    76    193    310    386    503    579    697    793    890    1007    1083    1200 

MD.  17.2;  S.D.  18.5 

ally,  but  erroneously,  ascribed  to  Schlick,  and,  according  to  Ellis, 
still  in  use  in  England  in  the  early  nineteenth  century.  The  G* 
is  now  almost  a  comma  sharper  than  in  the  pure  1/4-comma 


*Fora  condensed  version  of  the  material  in  this  chapter,  see  J.  Murray  Bar- 
bour, "Irregular  Systems  of  Temperament,"  Journal  of  the  American  Mu- 
sicological  Society,  I  (1948),  20-26. 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


temperament.  The  mean  deviation  is  noticeably  lower,  but  the 
standard  deviation  is  affected  less0 

Mersenne  has  included  a  discussion  of  the  meantone  temper- 
ament with  all  his  other  tuning  information.  His  account  differs 
slightly  in  the  different  works  where  it  occurs.  In  the  Harmonie 
universelle  (pp.  364  f.)  he  had  made  the  fifths  Eb-Bb-F  perfect. 
In  the  Cogitata  physico-mathematica  (p.  338)  he  asked  the  reader 
to  correct  the  "obvious  errors"  in  the  previous  description.  Here 
he  indicated  simply  that  the  wolf  fifth  will  be  G#-Eb.  Perhaps 
his  real  intent  is  to  be  found  in  Harmonicorum  libri  XH  (p.  60), 
where  these  two  fifths  are  to  be  sharp,  but  not  so  sharp  as  the 
wOlf  fifth,  which  is  still  unusable.  Mersenne  said  that  the  mean- 
tone  fifth  is  tempered  "1/136,  which  is  about  1/4  comma."  This 
is  a  gross  misstatement,  for  the  ratio  given  is  larger  than  1/2 
comma.  He  probably  meant  1/316,  which  is  a  reasonably  close 
value. 

Mersenne' s  improvements  upon  the  regular  meantone  tem- 
perament are  worth  showing,  even  if  the  second  will  be  only  an 
approximation  to  what  he  had  in  mind.  In  the  first  temperament 
(Table  111)  the  fifths  ED-Bb  and  Bb-F  are  pure.  For  the  second 
(Table  112),  note  that  the  excess  of  the  minor  third  G#(Ab)-F 
over  the  third  of  equal  temperament  is  30  cents.  Let  us  divide 
this  excess  so  that  G#-ED  bears  only  half  of  it,  the  other  two 
fifths  one -quarter  each. 

Table  111.    Mersenne's  Improved  Meantone  Temperament,  No.  1 

Names    C    C#     D       Eb       E        F       F#      G        G#       A        Bb        B  C 

Cents       0    76     193     299     386    503     579     697     773    890    1001     1083     1200 

M.D.  17.2;   S.D.  17.7 

j 

Table  112.    Mersenne's  Improved  Meantone  Temperament,  No.  2 

Names     C    C*     D       Eb       E        F       F#       G        G#      A        Bb         B  C 

Cents       0    76    193     288     386     503     579     697    773    890    996     1083     1200 

M.D.  15.3;   S.D.   16.9 


134 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


In  Mersenne's  first  improved  meantone  system,  the  mean  de- 
viation is  no  lower  than  for  the  temperament  previously  shown; 
but  the  standard  deviation  is  lower  because  more  notes  are  in- 
volved in  the  change.  Mersenne's  second  improvement  was  the 
pattern  for  a  modification  recommended  by  Rameau.  Now  Ra- 
meau  is  noted  chiefly  in  tuning  history  for  his  advocacy  of  equal 
temperament.  But  he  vacillated  sufficiently  in  his  adherence  to 
it  to  follow  Huyghens  in  acclaiming  as  "the  most  perfect  of  all" 
temperaments  that  in  which  "the  fifth  is  diminished  by  the  1/4 
part  of  a  comma. **  But  he  was  aware  of  the  pitfalls  of  the  mean- 
tone  temperament;  for  he  showed  that,  if  the  tuning  is  begun  on 
C,  G#  will  be  a  "minor  comma,"  2025/2048  too  flat.  The  remain- 
ing fifths,  therefore,  should  be  tuned  "more  just,"  "to  regain  the 
minor  comma  that  has  been  lost."  It  would  be  even  better  to  be- 
gin with  C*,  in  order  to  spread  the  discrepancy  over  more  notes. 

This  account  sounds  as  if  the  excess  should  be  divided  equally 
among  the  last  five  fifths.  But,  in  a  later  paragraph,  Rameau 
declared  that  "the  excess  of  the  last  two  fifths  and  of  the  last 
four  or  five  major  thirds  is  tolerable,  not  only  because  it  is  al- 
most insensible,  but  also  because  it  is  found  in  modulations  little 
used/  Apparently  the  first  three  of  the  five  fifths  are  not  to  be 
so  sharp  as  the  final  two  fifths.  Still  later  he  recommended  that 
"the  division  begin  on  B*3,  and  only  those  fifths  that  follow  B-F* 
should  be  a  little  more  just." 

These  directions  are  as  vague  as  Mersenne's.  In  Table  113 
the  division  is  begun  on  bP  as  Rameau  suggested.  The  fifths 
from  B  to  G*  have  been  made  pure,  and  the  excess  has  been  di- 
vided equally  between  G*-D*  and  E^-B^. 

Before  considering  a  final,  complicated  modification  of  the 
1/4-comma  temperament,  let  us  look  at  William  Hawkes'   im- 

Table  113.    Rameau's  Modified  Meantone  Temperament 

Names    C    C#     D      D#       E        F       F#      G       G#       A        Bb         B  C 

Cents      0    87    193    298    386    503    585    697    789    890    1007    1083    1200 

M.D.  12.5;  S.D.  14.0 

2J.  P.  Rameau,  Nouveau  systeme  de  musique  th^orique  (Paris,  1726),  pp.  107  ff. 

135 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


provement  upon  the  1/5-comma  temperament.  This  resembles 
Mersenne's  first  modification.  In  it,  according  to  John  Farey,^ 
"each  ascending  fifth  is  flattened  by  one-fifth  of  a  comma  as  the 
instrument  is  tuned,  except  that  the  fifth  above  E*3  and  the  fifth 
below  G^  are  directed  to  be  tuned  perfect."  Farey  continued: 
u. .  .but  why  these  anomalies  in  the  system  are  introduced  I  am 
at  a  loss  to  guess,  especially  as  G*  is  thereby  made  1/5  comma 
the  worse  by  it,"  Hawkes'  reason  is  perfectly  valid  —  to  dimin- 
ish the  wolf  fifth  by  2/5  comma,  although  it  will  still  be  16  cents 
sharp.  The  alteration  results  in  a  somewhat  smaller  deviation 
than  for  the  pure  1/5-comma  temperament. 

The  most  involved  of  all  these  temperaments  was  that  of  J.  E. 
Gallimard,4  who  brought  a  knowledge  of  logarithms  to  bear  upon 
the  problem,  in  order  to  obtain  a  subtly  modified  meantone  tem- 
perament. He  expressed  intervals  for  all  the  principal  tuning 
systems  in  Sauveur's  Decamerides  —  four-place  logarithms 
without  the  decimal  point.  The  first  of  his  original  tempera- 
ments used  the  values  of  the  1/4-comma  temperament  for  the 
eight  notes  from  B^  to  B,  If  Gallimard  had  continued  in  this 
fashion  until  the  entire  octave  had  been  tuned,  the  final  fifth 
(D  -B")  would  have  borne  the  usual  wolf,  amounting  to  103  Deca. 
He  split  up  this  error  by  adding  an  ever-increasing  amount  to 
each  logarithm  for  the  five  fifths  from  B  to  A*.  Thus  there  would 
be  a  total  of  1  +  2  +  3  +  4  +  5=15  parts  to  be  divided  into  103 
Deca.,  or  about  7  Deca.  for  each  part.  In  cents,  this  means  that 
the  first  seven  fifths  have  a  value  of  696  or  697  cents  each,  the 
others 699,  702,  705,  708,  710  cents  respectively.  Gallimard  has 
pure  thirds  in  all  the  principal  triads  of  the  keys  of  Fand  C,  and 
the  poorest  thirds  in  the  key  of  G^0  The  third  on  G^  itself  has 
425  cents,  practically  a  diesis  sharp! 

In  Gallimard' s  second  temperament,  the  first  eight  notes  were 
tuned  as  in  the  previous  temperament.  But  he  distributed  the 
error  among  the  other  five  fifths,  proportional  to  the  series  1, 
3,  6,  10,  15;  that  is,  to  the  series  n(n-l)/2.    The  cents  values  for 


3"On  Music,"  Philosophical  Magazine,  XXVI  (1806),  171-176. 
^L'arithm^tique  des  musiciens,  p.  26. 

136 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


these  altered  fifths  are  698,  700,  704,  708,  and  714»  Here  the 
worst  fifths  are  worse  than  in  his  first  temperament,  and  this 
error  is  reflected  in  a  slightly  higher  deviation.  His  worst  third, 
G^-B^,  is  still  a  diesis  sharp. 

The  deviations  are  still  large  for  Gallimard's  modification. 
Had  he  been  willing  to  use  a  modification  of  the  1/6-comma  tem- 
perament, with  slightly  sharp  diatonic  thirds,  his  system  would 
have  been  better.  Modifications  of  the  latter  temperament  are 
to  be  found  later  in  this  chapter,  by  Young  and  Mercadier. 

Arnold  Schlick' s  temperament^  deserves  special  honor,  for 
apparently  he  was  the  first  writer  in  any  country  to  describe  a 
temperament  for  each  note  of  the  chromatic  octave.  Shohe  Ta- 
naka  and  Hugo  Riemann  have  broadcast  the  erroneous  idea  that 
Schlick  founded  the  meantone  system.  The  former  spoke  of  the 
"exact  instructions"  that  Schlick  had  given,  and  added,  "In  exact 
language  this  will  mean  that  each  fifth  is  to  be  flattened  by  1/4 
comma.""  This  reads  well,  but  is  utter  nonsense  with  relation 
to  what  Schlick  actually  said.  In  place  of  "exact  instructions"  he 
gave  very  indefinite  rules  that  create  a  problem  for  us. 

Beginning  with  F  on  the  organ  manual,  the  fifth  F-C  is  to  be 
somewhat  flat.  This  same  rule  is  to  be  followed  in  tuning  the 
other  "claves  naturales"  by  fifths,  making  the  octaves  perfect. 
As  to  the  major  thirds,  Schlick  said  that  "although  they  will  all 
be  too  high,  it  is  necessary  to  make  the  three  thirds  C-E,  F-A, 
and  G-B  better, ...  as  much  as  the  said  thirds  are  better,  so  much 
will  G#  be  worse  to  E  and  B." 

The  tuning  of  the  black  keys  is  to  be  made  similarly,  tuning 
upward  by  flat  fifths  from  B  to  obtain  F*  and  C#,  and  tuning  down- 
ward from  F  to  obtain  Bb  and  ED.  The  semitone  between  G  and 
A  received  special  attention.  As  G*  it  was  needed  as  the  third 
above  E;  as  A13  it  was  also  needed  as  the  third  below  C.  So 
Schlick  suggested  a  mean  value  for  this  note,  directing  that  the 
fifth  AD-ED  is  to  be  somewhat  larger  than  a  perfect  fifth. 

Whatever  Schlick' s  system,  it  could  not  have  been  the  mean- 

kSpiegel    der    Orgelmacher    und   Organisten,    in   Monatshefte    fur    Musikge- 
schichte,  1869,  pp.  41  f. 

"Shohe  Tanaka,  in  Vierteljahrsschrift  fur  Musikwissenschaft,  VI  (1890),  62, 

137 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


tone  system  as  described  so  carefully  by  Tanaka;  for  it  lacks 
pure  thirds.  Schlick  said  definitely  that  "all  will  be  too  high." 
Not  even  the  diatonic  thirds  are  to  be  pure,  only  made  "better 
than  the  rest." 

What,  then,  was  Schlick' s  tuning  method?  All  that  can  be  said 
with  assurance  is  that  it  was  an  irregular  system,  lying  some- 
where between  meantone  and  equal  temperament.  We  cannot  hope 
to  reconstruct  it  exactly;  but  it  will  be  worth  while  to  give  some 
idea,  at  least,  of  what  it  was  like.  Let  us  assume  that  Schlick 
used  the  same  size  of  tempered  fifth  for  each  of  the  six  diatonic 
fifths;  a  somewhat  larger,  but  still  flat,  fifth  for  the  four  chro- 
matic fifths;  and  a  sharp  fifth  for  the  two  fifths  A^-E^  and  C*- 
G*.  Call  these  temperaments  x,  y,  and  -z  respectively.  Then, 
since  the  ditonic  comma  must  be  absorbed  in  the  course  of  the 
tuning, 

6x  +  4y  -  2z  =  24  cents. 

Now  x  is  larger  than  y;  let  us  assume  that  x  =  2y.  Since  Schlick 
said  that  most  of  his  fifths  were  to  be  "somewhat"  flat  and  the 
other  two  fifths  "somewhat"  sharp,  let  us  assume  that  x  =  z. 
Then 

12y  +  4y  -  4y  =  24  cents,  y  =  2  cents,  x  =  z  =  4  cents. 

Thus  Schlick' s  diatonic  fifths,  of  698  cents,  will  be  tempered  by 
1/6  comma;  his  chromatic  fifths,  of  700  cents,  will  be  the  same 
size  as  those  in  equal  temperament;  his  two  sharp  fifths  will  be 
of  706  cents.  His  diatonic  thirds  will  be  six  cents  sharp;  his 
chromatic  thirds,  8  or  10  cents;  the  thirds  E-G*  and  A^-C,  18 
cents  (not  unbearable);  and  the  "foreign"  thirds,  B-D#,  F#-A#, 
and  D^-F,  26  cents,  slightly  more  than  a  comma. 

The  deviations  for  Schlick' s  hypothetical  temperament  are 
less  than  half  as  large  as  those  for  the  modified  meantone  tem- 
perament that  Tanaka  wrongly  ascribed  to  him  —  the  first  tem- 
perament in  this  chapter.  His  is  a  good  system,  holding  its  own 
in  comparison  with  systems  that  were  proposed  two  or  three 
centuries  later.   Of  the  irregular  systems  discussed  in  the  first 


138 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


section  of  this  chapter,  Schlick's  is  superior  to  Mersenne's,  Ra- 
meau's,  Hawkes',  and  Gallimard's. 

Even  so,  Schlick's  system  is  not  so  good  as  that  of  Gram- 
mateus,  next  to  be  discussed.  Therefore  we  must  not  assume 
that  the  present  reconstruction  has  erred  on  the  side  of  Schlick. 
As  a  temperament,  it  has  far  greater  significance  for  us  than  if 
it  had  been  the  meantone  temperament,  with  two  sharp  fifths.  It 
is  an  indication  that  in  the  early  sixteenth  century  organ  temper- 
ament was  nearer  to  equal  temperament  than  it  generally  was 
for  centuries  after  this  time.  Schlick's  directions  have  the  added 
weight  that  they  represent  the  practice  of  an  actual  organist,  un- 
concerned with  mathematics  or  the  theories  of  the  ancient  Greeks. 

Modifications  of  Regular  Temperaments 

In  the  next  main  group  of  irregular  temperaments  the  diatonic 
notes  are  tuned  according  to  one  of  the  well-known  regular  tem- 
peraments and  then  each  tone  is  divided  equally  to  form  the  chro- 
matic notes.  The  oldest  and  best  of  them  was  that  of  Henricus 
Grammateus,  or  Heinrich  Schreyber  of  Erfurt.  Grammateus 
tuned  the  diatonic  notes  of  his  monochord  according  to  the  Pyth- 
agorean ratios.  But  when  it  came  to  the  black  keys,  the  "minor 
semitones,"  he  followed  a  different  procedure.  These  were 
formed  by  dividing  each  tone  into  two  equal  semitones  by  the  Eu- 
clidean method  for  finding  a  geometric  mean  proportional.  Gram- 
mateus had  a  figure  to  illustrate  the  construction.  Perhaps  he 
obtained  this  method  of  halving  intervals  directly  from  Euclid. 
But  he  may  have  owed  it  to  Faber  Stapulensis^  (Jacques  le 
Febvre),  who  had  shown  that  it  was  impossible  to  divide  a  ton^ 
numerically  into  two  equal  parts,  but  that  the  halving  of  any  in- 
terval could  be  accomplished  by  geometry.  At  any  rate,  Ber- 
mudo,  whose  one  tuning  method  was  identical  with  Grammateus', 
did  depend  upon  Faber  for  the  method  of  constructing  the  mean 
proportionals.    Faber  exerted  great  influence  upon  later  writers 

7"Arithmetica  applicirt  oder  gezogen  auff  die  edel  Kunst  musica,"  an  appen- 
dix to  his  Ayn  new  kunstlich  Buech  (Niirnberg,  1518)  a 

^Elementa  musicalia  (Paris,  1496). 

139 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  114 .   Hawkes'  Modified  1/5-Comma  Temperament 

U      7  2  +£  4  1  „6  1  7  3  2 

Names    C°C      'D"    Eb  5  E~  =    F+5    Fr*   G"  G#~5   A_1  Bb     5   B"1       C° 
Cents      0    83    195    303    390    502    586    698    785    893    1005    1088     1200 

M.D.  12.7;  S.D.  13.0 

Table  115.    Gallimard's  Modified  Meantone  Temperament,  No.  1 

Names    C  C#      D      D#     E         F        F#         G       G#        A       Bb  B  C 

Deca.      0    212   484    744    969    1263    1461    1747  1980  2232  2526  2716  3010 

Cents      0    84     193    297    386    504      582      696      789     890    1007  1083  1200 

M.D.  13.3;  S.D.  14.9 

Table  116.    Gallimard's  Modified  Meantone  Temperament,  No.  2 

Names    C  C#      D      D#      E         F        F#        G        G#  A  Bb        B  C 

Deca.      0   204   484    734    969    1263    1457    1747  1969  2232  2526  2716  3010 

Cents      0    81     193    293    386     504      581      696    785  890  1007  1083  1200 

M.D.  14.0;   S.D.  15.6 

Table  117.    Schlick's  Temperament  (Hypothetical) 

Names    C°  C#_1D"    Eb+<5  E~~3    Fe    F1"1^'^1"^"   B^s     B"      C° 
Cents      0    90    196    302    392    502    590    698    796    894    1002    1090    1200 

M.D.  8.0;  S.D.  8.6 

Table  118.  Grammateus'  Monochord  (Pythagorean  with  Mean  Semitones) 
Names  C°  Cr  2  D°  D*^  (Eb+2)  E°  F°  fHg°  G#_2A°  Bb+^  B?  C° 
Cents     0   102    204    306  408   498    600   702   804  906  1008  1110  1200 

M.D.  3.3;  S.D.  4.5 


140 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


who  attempted  to  solve  the  tuning  problem.  Especially  among 
mathematical  writers  who  dabbled  in  this  field,  Faber's  name 
was  held  in  something  of  the  same  esteem  as  that  of  Boethius. 

This  monochord  division  of  Grammateus  is  seen  to  be  of  a 
subtle  and  theoretical  nature.  It  is  equivalent  to  dividing  the 
Pythagorean  comma  equally  between  the  fifths  B-F#  and  B^-F. 
As  such,  it  is  identical  with  Marpurg's  tuning  K.  This  tuning 
may  have  been  used  in  practice,  but  hardly  by  anyone  who  was  ac- 
customed, like  Schlick,  to  tune  by  ear.  Note  that  it  was  presented 
as  a  method  not  for  fretted  instruments,  but  for  organs.  Gram- 
mateus said  in  his  introduction:  "There  follows  herewith  an 
amusing  reckoning  which  serves  the  art  of  song  called  music, 
and  from  such  reckoning  springs  the  division  of  the  monochord, 
from  which  will  then  be  taken  the  proportionate  length  and  width 
of  the  organ  pipes  after  the  opinion  of  Pythagoras." 

So  far  as  we  know,  Grammateus  was  the  earliest  writer  with 
a  method  for  finding  equal  semitones  as  applied  to  a  tuning  sys- 
tem. Of  course  only  ten  semitones  will  be  equal,  the  other  two 
being  twelve  cents  smaller.  Probably  many  men  who  later  spoke 
about  equal  semitones  on  the  lute  may  have  had  in  mind  some 
such  division,  perhaps  made  by  dividing  the  tones  arithmetically 
instead  of  geometrically. 

GanassF  had  a  method  for  obtaining  equal  semitones  on  the 
lute  and  viol  by  linear  divisions,  using  the  ratios  of  just  intona- 
tion for  his  basic  scale.  Although  he  described  his  procedure  in 
more  complicated  terms,  his  monochord  might  have  been  tuned 
as  follows:  with  A  the  fundamental,  form  the  minor  third  C  with 
the  ratio  6:5;  form  F  and  G  as  perfect  fourth  and  fifth  to  C  with 
the  respective  ratios  4:3  and  3:2;  divide  the  space  between  A  and 
C  into  three  equal  parts  for  B^  and  B;  divide  the  space  between 
C  and  F  into  five  equal  parts  for  C*,  D,  Eb,  and  E;  F*  will  be 
half  way  between  F  and  G,  and  G*  halfway  between  G  and  the  oc- 
tave A.  The  construction  will  be  even  easier  if  we  start  with  C: 
form  F  and  G  as  perfect  fourth  and  fifth  to  C;  divide  the  space 
between  C  and  F  into  five  equal  parts,  between  F  and  G  into  two 
equal  parts,  and  between  G  and  the  octave  C  into  five  equal  parts. 

9Sylvestro  Ganassi,  Regola  Rubertina.     Lettione    seconda  (1543);     ed.    Max 
Schneider  (Leipzig,  1924),  Chap.  IV. 

141 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


In  the  monochord  shown  in  Table  119,  the  lengths  and  ratios  have 
been  added  according  to  Ganassi's  directions. 

Actually,  the  above  monochord  does  not  quite  represent  Ga- 
nassi's ideas.  His  lute  had  only  eight  frets,  so  that  the  position 
of  the  notes  above  F  is  rather  conjectural.  However,  he  placed 
a  dot  where  G,  the  tenth  fret,  would  naturally  fall,  and  it  is  rea- 
sonable to  suppose  that  he  would  have  made  a  linear  division  for 
the  semitones  on  either  side  of  G.  A  greater  departure  from  his 
ideas  lies  in  ignoring  the  tempering  of  the  first  and  second  frets: 
the  second  fret  is  to  be  placed  higher  than  8/9  by  the  width  of  the 
fret,  and  the  first  fret  higher  than  17/18  by  half  the  width  of  the 
freto  Similarly  the  sixth  fret  is  to  be  placed  lower  than  17/24  by 
the  width  of  the  fret.  His  drawing  for  the  monochord  is  made 
with  unusual  care  (see  Figure  H).  It  appears  as  if  the  width  of 
the  fret  were  about  1/2  of  1  percent  of  the  length  of  the  string. 
This  tempering  would  make  B*3  and  B  sharper  by  about  half  a 
comma,  and  E*3  flatter  by  the  same  amount.  The  first  two  changes 
would  not  affect  the  tuning  greatly,  but  the  change  in  the  position 
of  the  sixth  fret  would  be  harmful.  Since  Ganassi  was  not  spe- 
cific as  to  the  relative  length  and  breadth  of  the  string,  we  merely 
indicate  here  that  he  advocated  these  three  tempered  values. 


Fig.  H.   Ganassi's  Method  for  Placing  Frets  on  the  Lute  and  Viol 
Reproduced  by  courtesy  of  the  Library  of  Congress 


142 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


Table  119.  Ganassi's  Monochord  (Just  with  Mean  Semitones) 
Lengths  120  114  108  102  96  90  85 
Ratios     19/20    18/19    17/18    16/17   15/16   17/18   16/17 


Names 

C° 

X 

D"1 

X 

E"1 

F° 

X 

Cents 

0 

88 

182 

281 

386 

498 

597 

Lengths 

80 

76 

72 

68 

64 

60 

Ratios     19/20    18/19    17/18    16/17   15/16 
Names   G°     x       A"1      x      B"1     C° 
Cents    702    790     884      983    1088     1200 

M.D.  6.5;  S.D.  7.8 


Table  120.    Reinhard's  Monochord  (Variant  of  Ganassi's) 


Lengths 

60 

56  2/3 

53  1/3 

50  2/3 

48 

45 

42  1/2 

40 

38 

Names 

C° 

X 

D° 

X 

E'1 

F° 

X 

G° 

X 

Cents 

0 

99 

204 

292 

386 

498 

597 

702 

790 

Lengths 

36 

34 

32 

30 

Names 

A"1 

X 

B"1 

C° 

Cents 

884 

983 

1088 

1200 

M.D.  6.5;   S.D.  7.8 


Ratios 

Names 

C° 

Cents 

0 

Ratios 

Names 

G° 

Cents 

702 

Table  121.    Malcolm's  Monochord  (Variant  of  Ganassi's) 

16/17        17/18       18/19       19/20       15/16       16/17       17/18 


x  D°  x  E_1  F° 

105  204  298  386  498 

18/19        19/20       16/17       17/18       15/16 

x  A"1  x  B-1  C° 

796  884  989         1088  1200 

M.D.  6.5;   S.D.  7.8 


x 
603 


143 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Except  for  the  arithmetical  divisions,  Ganassi's  tuning  re- 
sembles Grammateus'  treatment  of  the  Pythagorean  tuning,  the 
difference  being  that  the  basic  scale  here  is  just  intonation.  It 
also  resembles  Artusi's  treatment  of  the  meantone  temperament, 
shortly  to  be  described.  But  even  if  Ganassi  had  used  the  Eu- 
clidean method  to  divide  his  tones,  his  monochord(M.D.  6.0;  S.D. 
7.3)  would  have  been  inferior  to  either  of  the  other  two,  since  the 
diatonic  just  scale  varies  more  greatly  from  equal  temperament 
than  either  the  Pythagorean  or  meantone  does.  But  this  is  a  good 
division,  and  has  the  tremendous  advantage  that  it  is  the  easiest 
of  all  chromatic  monochords  to  form. 

Ganassi's  method  was  discovered  independently  by  Andreas 
Reinhard,^  who  described  the  syntonic  tuning,  and  then  gave  a 
table  in  which  the  space  of  each  tone,  whether  major  or  minor, 
is  halved  to  obtain  the  chromatic  note.  His  table  gave  string- 
lengths  only,  beginning  with  45  for  F.  Since  he  used  D°  instead 
of  D"1 ,  his  intervals  are  in  a  slightly  different  order  from  Ga- 
nassi's. 

Ten  years  after  Reinhard,  his  tuning  method  was  taken  over 
by  Abraham  Bartolus,  *  the  sole  difference  being  that  the  latter 
began  with  E  (48)  instead  of  F  (45).  Bartolus  gave  Reinhard  as 
his  source.  At  first  he  advocated  the  method  for  keyboard  in- 
struments, and  later  prescribed  it  also  for  fretted  instruments 
and  bells.  This  general  application  of  a  tuning  method  is  some- 
thing that  is  found  in  very  few  theorists  of  Bartolus'  period,  most 
of  whom  continued  to  say  with  Vicentinothat  fretted  instruments 
used  equal  temperament,  and  keyboard  instruments,  the  mean- 
tone  temperament. 

In  one  of  the  curious  dialogs  of  Printz'  s  Phrynis  Mytilenaeus-^ 
this  same  temperament  is  mentioned.  "Charis"  describes  it  and 
gives  the  string-lengths  for  the  C  octave,  360  to  180,  thus  avoid- 
ing the  fractions  that  Reinhard  had  encountered.  Very  likely 
Printz  intended  this  for  Reinhard' s  tuning,  but  his  perplexing  use 

^"Monochordum  (Leipzig,  1604). 

11Musica  mathematica:    the  2nd  part  of  Heinrich  Zeising's  Theatri  machi- 
narum  (Altenburg,  1614),  pp.  151  f,  165 ff. 

12Part  3,  Chap.  6. 
144 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


of  anagrams  effectively  conceals  Reinhard's  name,  if  it  is  indeed 
hidden  there. 

Alexander  Malcolm**  had  a  division  very  similar  to  those  of 
Ganassi  and  Reinhard.  In  fact,  it  is  the  inversion  of  Ganassi's, 
with  semitones  paired  in  contrary  motion.  Although  Malcolm 
said  that  the  tones  were  to  be  divided  arithmetically,  as  16:17:18, 
his  table  of  string-lengths  (lengths  of  chords)  represents  a  very 
unlikely  division,  difficult  to  make.  Marpurg,  who  called  the 
system  ugly,  has  represented  it  by  a  series  of  increasing  num- 
bers, as  C,  C%  D  are  48,  51,  54.  This  would  mean  that  Mal- 
colm's ratios  are  to  be  taken  as  vibration  numbers,  improbable 
in  view  of  his  own  terminology  for  them. 

Since  Malcolm's  scale  contains  the  same  ratios  for  semitones 
as  Ganassi's  and  Reinhard's,  although  in  a  different  order,  the 
deviation  for  the  three  scales  will  be  the  same.  But  his  chro- 
matic notes  are  all  five  or  six  cents  higher  than  Reinhard's.  It 
is  very  probable  that  Malcolm  intended  the  same  division  as 
Reinhardo  Malcolm  stated  that  Thomas  Salmon  had  written  about 
this  scale.  But  it  is  often  referred  to  by  Malcolm's  name  alone. 
Certainly  these  well-nigh  equal  semitones  of  Ganassi,  Reinhard, 
Bartolus,  Salmon,  and  Malcolm  represent  a  long-lived  (almost 
two  centuries)  and  very  good  way  to  divide  the  octave  with  ease. 

Levens'  "Sisteme""  also  had  linear  divisions  only,  but  was 
far  less  successful  than  those  just  described.  His  monochord 
had  integer  numbers  starting  with  48  for  C.  Ganassi's  system 
had  only  five  consecutive  semitones  formed  by  equal  divisions 
of  a  larger  interval,  but  Levens'  had  seven,  from  42  for  D  to  28 
for  A.  Thus  Levens'  consecutive  semitones  vary  in  size  from  85 
to  119  cents.  Furthermore,  his  semitone  A-B"  is  very  small 
(63  cents),  with  the  Archytas  ratio,  28:27;  whereas  his  semitone 
Bb-Cb,  with  the  ratio  27:25,  is  more  than  twice  as  large  (133 
cents) .  Levens'  deviations  are  as  great  as  for  some  varieties  of 
just  intonation. 


13A  Treatise  of  Musick  (Edinburgh,  1721),  p.  304. 
Abrege'  des  regies  de  l'harmonie  (Bordeaux,  1743),  p.  87. 

145 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Since  C  is  48  in  Levens'  tuning,  the  monochord  could  easily 
be  constructed  with  a  foot  rule.  But  it  would  not  be  so  easy  to 
construct  a  monochord  of  indefinite  length  for  this  tuning.  A 
slight  change  in  the  values  of  A  and  B  would  greatly  simplify  the 
construction  of  the  monochord,  and  at  the  same  time  would  al- 
most cut  the  deviation  in  half.  It  would  then  be  formed  thus:  Di- 
vide the  entire  string  into  8  parts,  putting  D  at  the  first  point  of 
division,  F  at  the  second,  and  AD  at  the  third.  Divide  the  space 
between  C  and  D  into  two  parts  for  C*.  Divide  the  space  between 
D  and  F  into  three  parts,  for  E"  and  E,  and  apply  EF  twice  from 
F  toward  A13,  for  F*  and  G.  Divide  the  space  from  A*5  to  the 
higher  C  (midpoint  of  the  string)  into  four  equal  parts,  for  A,  B", 
and  B. 

The  third  distinct  method  of  forming  equal  semitones  upon 
the  lute  stems  from  Giovanni  Maria  Artusi.  **  But,  as  with  Gram- 
mateus'  division,  only  ten  of  the  semitones  would  be  equal.  In 
pointing  out  the  "errors  of  certain  modern  composers,"  Artusi 
gave  two  examples  of  "intervals  false  for  singing,  but  good  for 
playing  on  the  lute."  Thus  the  diminished  seventh,  C*-B",  in  the 
beginning  of  Marenzio's  madrigal  "False  Faith,"  is  "false  for 
voices  and  for  modulation,  but  not  false  on  the  lute  and  the 
chitarone." 

On  the  lute,  he  continued,  "the  tone  is  divided  into  two  equal 
semitones."  So  far  Artusi  had  been  speaking  very  much  as  had 
his  predecessors.  But  he  then  stated  that  the  tone  in  question  is 
not  the  9:8  tone,  but  the  mean  tone  used  on  the  lute  and  other  in- 
struments. Later  he  called  the  tempered  semitone  "the  just  half 
of  the  mean  tone."  For  constructing  this  temperament  he  men- 
tioned the  mesolabium  and  the  Euclidean  construction  for  a  mean 
proportional,  with  references  to  Zarlino  and  Faber,  The  meso- 
labium would  have  been  useless  for  this  purpose,  unless  Artusi 
had  desired  complete  equal  temperament.  But  Euclid's  method 
would  have  served  for  constructing  meantones  from  just  major 
thirds,  and  then  for  constructing  mean  semitones  from  mean  tones. 

Since  Artusi  did  not  give  a  detailed  account  of  how  his  tem- 
perament was  to  be  formed,  we  can  only  surmise  that  all  the  di- 

l^Seconda  parte  dell'  Artusi  overo  della  imperfettioni  della  moderna  musica 
(Venice,  1603),  pp.  30  ff. 

146 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


Table  122.  Levens'  Monochord  (Linear  Divisions) 

Lengths  48  45   42   40   38   36  34   32  30   28  27  25  24 

Names   C°  Db+1  D   Eb+1  E   F°  x   G°  Ab+1  A  Bbo  Cb+2  C° 

Cents    0   112  231  316  404  498  597  702  814  933  996  1129  1200 

M.D.  16.7;  S.D.  19.9 

Table  123.    Levens'  Monochord  (Altered  Form) 

Lengths  48    45      42     40      38     36      34     32      30    28  1/2    27     25  1/2  24 

Names     C°   Db+1  D     Eb+1  E      F°       x      G°     Ab+1      A       Bb°        B  C° 

Cents        0     112    231   316   404  498    597  702    814      902      996     1095  1200 

M.D.  8.8;  S.D.  10.3 

Table  124.    Artusi's  Monochord  (Meantone  with  Mean  Semitones) 
(Bonded  Clavichord  Tuning,  No.  1) 

_  1  j_l  1  3  5 

Names  C°  x  D  f   x  E~      F  5   x   G  ?   x   A"   x    B~4   C° 
Cents   0  97  193  290  386  503  600  697  794  890  987  1083  1200 

M.D.  5.7;  S.D.  7.6 

Table  125.    Bonded  Clavichord  Tuning,  No.  2 

_i  _2  .1  _1  _i  _5 

Names     C°    x     D   3      x      E   3    F  6      x      G  6      x      A   2       x        B   6       C° 
Cents       0    97     197    294     394     502     599     698    795    895     992     1092     1200 

M.D.  2.6;   S.D.  3.8 


147 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


atonic  notes  were  to  be  tuned  as  in  the  ordinary  meantone  tem- 
perament and  the  chromatic  notes  by  dividing  each  of  the  tones 
in  half.  This  is  the  "semi-meantone  temperament"  mentioned 
by  Ellis,  I®  "in  which  the  natural  notes  C,  D,  E,  F,  G,  A,  B  were 
tuned  in  meantone  temperament,  and  the  chromatics  were  inter- 
polated at  intervals  of  half  a  meantone."  According  to  Ellis,  it 
had  been  in  use  on  "the  old  fretted  or  bonded  clavichords."  Un- 
fortunately, Ellis  did  not  give  the  source  of  this  information. 

If  these  bonded  clavichords  had  had  their  notes  paired  CC* 
DD*  E  FF#  GG#  AA*  B  C,  a  fixed  ratio  could  have  existed  be- 
tween the  notes  in  each  pair,  so  that  C#,  for  example,  would  always 
be  96.5  cents  higher  than  C.  Of  course,  the  two  diatonic  semi- 
tones, E-F  and  B-C,  would  be  about  a  comma  larger,  at  117 
cents  each. 

Some  writers  have  said  that  the  bonded  clavichords  neces- 
sarily used  the  meantone  temperament.  But  nothing  would  have 
prevented  the  performer  from  tuning  his  diatonic  tones  sharper 
than  mean  tones.  Suppose,  for  example,  it  had  become  the  fashion 
to  diminish  the  fifth  by  1/^  comma,  as  in  Bach's  day.  Then  the 
bonded  clavichord  would  have  had  the  scale  shown  in  Table  125. 

In  this  tuning  the  standard  deviation  is  fairly  large  because 
the  semitones  E-F  and  B-C  have  a  deviation  of  eight  cents,,  If  E 
and  B  are  made  four  cents  sharper,  the  mean  deviation  is  un- 
changed, but  the  standard  deviation  is  reduced  to  3.0.  This  much 
can  be  done  without  changing  the  ratio  of  C  to  C*.  But  a  bonded 
clavichord  that  was  constructed  at  the  time  Douwes  was  writing 
(1699;  see  Chapter  in)  would  have  had  the  ratio  of  this  pair  of 
notes  fixed  according  to  the  temperament  then  in  use,  perhaps 
the  1/6-comma  meantone  system,  and  the  mean-semitone  tuning 
would  then  have  been  even  better  than  in  Table  125. 

Furthermore,  there  is  no  valid  reason  why  the  ratio  of  the 
semitones  on  a  single  string  could  not  have  been  -J2,  if  the  bonded 
clavichord  had  been  constructed  at  a  time  when  equal  tempera- 
ment was  widely  accepted.  The  only  difficulty  is  that  the  free 
clavichords  were  more  common  then.    But  it  is  nonsense  to  think 


^Alexander  Ellis,  "On  the  History  of  Musical  Pitch,"  Journal  of  the  Society 
of  Arts,  XXVIII  (1880),  295. 

148 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


that  there  was  any  connection  between  free  clavichords  and  equal 
temperament,  except  where  an  old  clavichord  had  retained  sem- 
itonal  ratios  that  belonged  to  a  type  of  tuning  that  had  been  su- 
perseded. Even  then,  as  we  have  shown,  the  open  strings  could 
have  been  tuned  so  that  the  instrument  as  a  whole  would  have 
varied  only  slightly  from  equal  temperament. 

The  only  troublesome  situation  would  occur  when  the  bonded 
clavichord  had  its  ratios  fixed  so  that,  for  example,  the  semitone 
between  C°  and  D"^  was  not  a  mean  semitone,  but  C#~^.  Re- 
member that  Artusi  was  writing  about  equal  semitones  on  the 
lute,  not  on  the  clavichord.  And  other  theorists,  advocating 
meantone  temperament  for  keyboard  instruments,  made  no  dis- 
tinction between  the  clavichord,  on  one  hand,  and  the  organ  and 
harpsichord,  on  the  other.  Let  us  see,  in  Table  126,  what  could 
be  done  when  the  fixed  chromatic  semitone  has  only  76  cents,  the 
diatonic  semitone,  117  cents. 

Here  we  assume  that  C-C#,  F-F#,and  G-G*are  each  76  cents, 
and  that  D-E^andA-B^are  each  117  cents.  The  other  seven  sem- 
itones are  free.  If  we  make  them  all  equal,  each  will  have  105.4 
cents.  That  means  that  D  and  A  are  flatter  than  in  the  regular 
meantone  temperament;  E,  F,  G,  and  B  sharper.  After  this 
somewhat  eccentric  tuning  of  the  diatonic  notes,  the  deviation  is 
almost  half  that  of  the  regular  meantone  temperament,  but  is 
still  not  quite  so  good  as  that  of  the  old  Pythagorean  tuning,  un- 
tempered.  Therefore  on  a  bonded  clavichord  that  was  built  for 
the  complete  meantone  temperament,  even  the  most  scientific 
tuning  of  the  free  strings  would  not  make  a  very  acceptable  tem- 
perament. And  such  clavichords  would  certainly  have  delayed 
the  acceptance  of  equal  temperament. 

A  corroboration  of  Artusi' s  method  of  forming  equal  semi- 
tones on  the  lute  came  from  Ercole  Bottrigari.*'  He  had  clas- 
sified instruments  by  their  tuning,  as  Zarlino  had  done.  He  went 
on  to  show  that  the  lute  cannot  play  in  tune  with  the  cembalo.  If 
the  E  string  of  the  lute  is  tuned  in  unison  with  the  E  of  the  cem- 


*'I1  dcsiderio,  ovvero  de'  concerti  di  varii  stromenti  musicali  (Venice,  1594); 
new  ed.  by  Kathi  Meyer    (Berlin,  1924). 

149 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  126.  Bonded  Clavichord  Tuning,  No.  3 

Names  C  C*  D   Eb   E    F  F#      G   G#   A   Bb   B     C 
Cents   0  76  181  298  403  509  585  691  767  872  989  1094  1200 

M.D.  12.0;  S.D.  13.7 

balo,  the  F's  will  be  out  of  tune,  the  G's  will  again  be  in  tune,  and 
the  G^'s  out  of  tune.  He  explained  that,  since  on  the  lute  the  tone 
was  divided  into  two  equal  semitones,  and  on  the  cembalo  into 
two  unequal  semitones,  then  the  diatonic  semitone  E-F,  with  the 
ratio  of  16:15  tempered,  would  be  higher  on  the  cembalo  than  on 
the  lute;  but  the  chromatic  semitone  G-G*  (25:24  tempered)  would 
be  higher  on  the  lute. 

This  explanation  would  be  true,  even  if  the  lute  were  in  equal 
temperament.  But  the  interesting  question  is  why  the  G's  were 
in  tune  if  the  E's  were,  and  vice  versa.  If  the  lute  were  in  equal 
temperament,  it  would  have  no  pitches  in  unison  with  the  cem- 
balo save  the  one  that  was  tuned  to  a  unison  to  begin  with.  Now, 
Bottrigari  was  referring  to  a  tuning  in  which  the  order  of  strings 
was  D,  G,  C,  E,  A,  D.  Of  these  the  E  string  was  called  the  "me- 
zanina,"  the  middle  string.  On  either  D  string  or  on  the  A  string, 
the  2nd,  3rd,  and  5th  frets  formed  a  diatonic  sequence  —  A,  B, 
C,  D  or  D,  E,  F,  G. 

Since  the  position  of  the  frets  was  the  same  on  all  the  strings, 
the  succession  on  the  E  string  would  have  been  E,  F*,  G,  A. 
Therefore,  if  the  diatonic  notes  on  the  D  and  A  strings  were  tuned 
in  unison  with  those  on  the  cembalo,  as  in  Artusi's  tuning,  the 
notes  E,  F%  G,  and  A  on  the  E  string  will  also  be  in  unison.  But 
E-F  on  the  lute  will  behalf  a  mean  tone  and  so  will  G-G^,  whereas 
the  E-F  of  the  cembalo  will  be  a  tempered  major  semitone  and 
the  G-G*  a  tempered  minor  semitone.  (F#-G,  about  which  Bot- 
trigari said  nothing,  will  be  the  ordinary  major  semitone  of  the 
meantone  temperament  on  both  instruments,  and  will  be  almost 
a  comma  larger  than  these  other  semitones  on  the  lute.)  This  is 
the  only  reasonable  explanation  of  Bottrigari' s  statement,  and, 
since  it  was  made  only  nine  years  earlier  than  Artusi's  account, 
we  may  surmise  that  this  method  of  tuning  was  in  common  use 
about  1600.    We  should  be  careful,  therefore,  not  to  assume  that 

150 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


every  statement  about  the  use  of  equal  semitones  on  the  lute  nec- 
essarily meant  equal  temperament,  with  the  ratio  of1  {2  for  the 
semitone. 

Temperaments  Largely  Pythagorean 

A  great  many  irregular  temperaments  are  largely  Pythagor- 
ean, that  is,  they  contain  many  pure  fifths.  This  is  reasonable 
enough,  since  pure  fifths  are  easy  to  tune  and  do  not  depart 
greatly  from  the  fifths  of  equal  temperament.  As  we  shall  see, 
many  of  these  are  typical  "paper"  temperaments,  ill  adapted 
either  to  tuning  by  ear  or  to  setting  upon  a  monochord.    But  first 

we  shall  examine  several  that  used  linear  divisions  only. 
1ft 
Martin  Agricola,  °    who  was  responsible  for  a  good  version 

of  just  intonation,  showed  a  monochord  for  the  lute  in  which  the 
diatonic  notes,  like  those  of  Grammateus,  were  joined  by  pure 
fifths.  To  divide  the  tones  into  diatonic  and  chromatic  semitones, 
Agricola  applied  the  old  doctrine  that  the  tone  is  divisible  into  9 
commas,  5  for  the  chromatic  semitone  and  4  for  the  diatonic. 
He  tuned  a  G  string,  marking  off  G*  as  5/9  the  distance  from  G 
to  A.  That  means  that  G:G#:A  as  81:76:72.  Thus  the  diatonic 
semitone  G*-A  had  the  ratio  19:18,  or  almost  94  cents,  instead 
of  256:243  or  90  cents,  and  the  chromatic  semitone  110  cents  in- 
stead of  114. 

Agricola  formed  his  A*  and  C*  like  the  G*.  As  there  were 
only  seven  frets  on  this  string,  he  did  not  give  values  for  D#,  F, 
and  F#.  But  F  is  of  course  a  major  tone  below  G,  and  he  had 
previously  shown  ED  (although  he  called  it  "dis")  to  be  a  tone  be- 
low F.  But  there  B^  had  been  shown  to  be  a  tone  lower  than  C, 
20  cents  flatter  than  the  A*  on  the  other  string.  These  incon- 
sistencies are  bound  to  arise  when  any  unequal  tuning  is  used  on 
a  fretted  instrument,  as  Galilei  pointed  out.  For  the  sake  of  a 
logical  construction,  let  us  assume  (see  Table  127)  that  each  of 
the  five  tones  in  the  octave  is  divided  into  5  +  4  commas.  This 
may  be  slightly  better  than  Agricola' s  tuning  would  have  been  if 

18Musica  instrumentalis  deudsch  (4th  ed.;  Wittenberg,  1545).  Reprinted  as 
Band  20  of  Publikation  'alterer  praktischer  und  theoretischer  Musikwerke, 
1896.    The  reference  here  is  to  page  227  of  the  latter. 

151 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  127.    Agricola's  Pythagorean- Type  Monochord 

Names    C°C#~5D°    D#"6    E°     F°      F^G0      G^"5  A0      A#_1      B°        C° 
Cents      0    110    204    314     408    498     608     702    812    906     1016     1110    1200 

M.D.  8.3;   S.D.  8.6 

Table  128     WSng  Pho's  Pythagorean- Type  Monochord 

Lengths    900   844    800    751    713    668    633    600  563  534  501  475  450 

Names        C       C*      D      D*       E       E#     F#      G  G*      A  A#         B  C 

Cents  0      111    204    313    403    516    609    702  812  904  1014  1107  1200 

M.D.  8.9;  S.D.  9.0 

he  had  applied  it  to  an  entire  octave. 

This  system,  if  we  can  call  it  a  system,  is  appreciably  better 
than  the  ordinary  Pythagorean  tuning.  It  contains  ten  pure  fifths; 
the  fifth  B-F#  is  four  cents  flat  (1/6  comma),  and  A#-F  is  twenty 
cents  flat.  But  none  of  the  credit  belongs  to  the  inventor.  Agric- 
ola,  like  many  another  good  man,  confused  geometrical  with  arith- 
metical proportion  The  old  statement  about  the  sizes  of  semi- 
tones is  very  nearly  correct  when  geometrical  magnitudes  are 
in  question,  but  is  less  accurate  when  applied  to  linear  divisions. 
Furthermore,  it  was  a  happy  accident  that  led  him  to  make  his 
chromatic  notes  sharps.  If  he  had  divided  the  tone  G-A  into  G- 
A^-Ainthis  same  manner,  his  diatonic  semitone  would  have  con- 
tained 88  cents,  the  chromatic,  116,  thus  diverging  more  widely 
from  equality  than  the  Pythagorean  semitones  do.  An  accidental 
improvement  is  the  best  we  can  say  for  this  tuning  of  Agricola. 

Agricola's  approximation  for  the  Pythagorean  tuning  suggests 
the  monochord  of  an  early  Chinese  theorist,  W&ng  Ph5,  who  lived 
toward  the  end  of  the  tenth  century. ^  Perhaps  he  was  familiar 
with  the  excellent  temperament  of  Ho  Tchheng-thyen,  but,  if  so, 
was  too  timid  to  follow  his  example.  Starting  with  the  Pytha- 
gorean tuning  for  the  octave  900-450,  he  has  retained  the  purity 

^Maurice  Courant,  in  Encyclopedic  de  la  musique  et  dictionnaire  du  conser- 
vatoire, Part  1,  Vol.  I,  p.  90. 

152 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


of  G  and  D.  He  lowered  the  pitches  of  all  the  other  notes  by  add- 
ing two  units  for  C#,  D*,  E,  and  E#,  and  one  unit  for  F#,  G#,  A, 
A*,  and  B.  This  was  too  small  a  correction  for  most  of  the  notes, 
as  can  be  seen  from  Table  128,  which  is  comparable  to  that  of 
Agricola. 

John  Dowland  is  another  writer  whose  tuning  system,  like 
those  of  Ramis,  Grammateus,  Agricola,  and  others,  had  a  strong 
Pythagorean  cast.  In  his  account  of  "fretting  the  lute,"  C,  D,  F, 
G,  and  A  have  Pythagorean  tuning.20  The  chromatic  semitone 
from  C  to  C#  is  33:31,  or  108  cents,  not  far  from  the  Pythago- 
rean of  114  cents.  The  diatonic  semitone  from  D  to  E^  is  22:21,  or 
80  cents,  considerably  flatter  than  the  Pythagorean  of  90  cents. 
G*  and  B*3  form  pure  fifths  to  C#  and  E*5  respectively.  An  unu- 
sual feature  of  the  tuning  is  F*  taken  as  the  arithmetical  mean  be- 
tween F  and  G,  and  E  (!)asthe  mean  between  E^°  and  F.  The  value 
for  E  thus  obtained,  264:211,  is  388  cents,  almost  a  pure  third  above 
C,  instead  of  the  expected  Pythagorean  third.  The  third  D-F#,  of 
393  cents,  is  likewise  an  improvement.  Thus  the  deviation  is 
somewhat  less  than  that  for  the  Pythagorean  tuning,  being  almost 
the  same  as  that  of  Agricola' s  system.  There  is  no  B  on  this 
string,  but  we  have  made  B  a  pure  fifth  above  E. 

The  trend  of  Dowland' s  tuning  resembles  that  of  Ornithopar- 
chus,  whose  Micrologus  was  translated  into  English  by  Dowland. 
Ornithoparchus'  division  of  the  monochord  was  entirely  Pytha- 
gorean, a  ten-note  system  extending  fromA^  to  B  by  pure  fifths. 
It  was  natural  for  Ornithoparchus  to  advocate  the  Pythagorean 
tuning,  since  most  of  his  contemporaries  had  not  yet  departed 
from  it„  But  a  century  later,  the  Pythagorean  tuning  was  becom- 
ing somewhat  rare.  And  yet  Dowland' s  fellow  countryman  Thomas 
Morley,  whose  precepts  have  been  quoted  by  everyone  who  writes 
about  Elizabethan  music,  gave  only  a  Pythagorean  monochord. 

Unusual  ratios  are  a  feature  of  Colonna's  tunings  also,  al- 
though he  definitely  included  some  ratios  that  belong  to  just  in- 
tonation as  well.21    He  is  noted  in  the  field  of  multiple  division 

20Robert  Dowland,  Variety  of  Lute-Lessons  (London,  1610).  "Of  Fretting  the 
Lute"  comes  under  "Other  Necessary  Observations  to  Lute-playing  by  John 
Dowland,  Bachelor  of  Music." 

^^Fabio  Colonna,  La  sambuca  lincea,  p.  22. 

153 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


for  having  described  an  instrument,  theSambucaLincea,  similar 
to  Vicentino's  Archicembalo,  upon  which  the  division  of  the  oc- 
tave into  §1  parts  could  be  accomplished.  His  mathematical  the- 
ory of  intervals  is  very  ingenious,  including  superparticular  pro- 
portions, but  also  more  subtle  fractions.  He  began  with  certain 
well-known  consonant  ratios:  1:1  (unison),  6:5  (minor  third),  5:4 
(major  third),  4:3  (fourth),  3:2  (fifth),  and  5:3  (major  sixth).  Then 
if  a  string  of  the  monochord  is  divided  to  produce  a  certain  in- 
terval, the  sounding  part  of  the  string  should  produce  with  the 
other  part  (the  Residuo)  either  one  of  the  above  intervals  or  a 
higher  octave  of  it.  This  means  that  if  any  of  the  above  ratios  is 
called  b:a,  intervals  derived  from  it  have  ratios  of  the  form 
(2*b  +  a):2^b.  For  example,  from  1:1  comes  17:16;  from  6:5 
comes  11:6;  from  3:2  comes  25:24.  Colonna's  two  chromatic 
monochords  are  shown  in  Tables  130  and  131.  Each  contains 
seven  pure  fifths  and  several  pure  thirds.  The  worst  feature  of 
both  monochords  is  the  55:54  chromatic  semitone  of  30  cents  (as 
G-G^  or  B^-B)  —  not  much  larger  than  a  comma.  Almost  as  bad 
is  the  12:11  diatonic  semitone  of  152  cents,  as  G*-A  or  B-C.  * 
The  27:25  diatonic  semitone  of  134  cents,  as  F*~  -G  or  C*  - 
D° ,  is  not  good  either,  but  is  a  blemish  found  also  in  ordinary 
just  intonation.  A  redeeming  feature  of  the  first  monochord  is 
the  division  of  the  9:8  tone  into  17:16  and  18:17  semitones. 

Colonna's  division  of  the  10:9  tone  into  12:11  and  55:54  "sem- 
itones" is  reminiscent  of  the  superparticular  division  of  the  10:9 
tone  that  Ptolemy  used  for  his  soft  chromatic  tetrachord,  5/f5  x 
14/15  x  27/28,  and  of  the  common  division  of  just  intonation  de- 
rived from  Didymus'  chromatic,  5/6  x  24/25  x  15/16. ^   Other 


22Henri  Louis  Choquel  used  a  12:11  semitone  between  A  and  Bb  and  a  33:32 
semitone  between  BD  and  B,  in  what  was  otherwise  a  monochord  in  ordinary 
just  intonation.  La  musique  rendue  sensible  par  la  me'chanique  (New  ed., 
Paris,  1762). 

2*\A.  m,  Awraamoff  in  1920  devised  a  tuning  for  the  chromatic  octave  that  out- 
does Colonna's.  The  natural  seventh,  8:7,  is  exploited  in  this  tuning,  and 
such  superparticular  near-commatic  intervals  occur  in  it  as  49:48  (36  cents) 
and  64:63  (27  cents)!  "Jenseits  von  Temperierung  und  Tonalitat,"  Melos, 
Vol.  I  (1920). 

154 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


Table  129.    Dowland's  Lute  Tuning 


Ratios 

1 

33:31 

9:8 

33:28 

264:211 

4:3 

24:17 

3:2 

Names 

C 

C* 

D 

Eb 

X 

F 

X 

G 

Cents 

0 

108 

204 

284 

388 

498 

597 

702 

99:62 


810 


Ratios    27:16    99:56  [396:21l]    2:1 
Names       A         Bb  x  C 

Cents       906        986        1090       1200 

M.D.  8.2;  S.D.  10.1 

Table  130.    Colonna's  Irregular  Just  Intonation,  No.  1 

Lengths       50  48  45         [42  6/17]       40         371/2  36 

Ratios  24/25       15/16       16/17       17/18       15/16       24/25       25/27 

Names         C°  C¥~2         D"1  [Eb]  E"1  F°  F*-2 

Cents  0  70  182  287  386  498  568 

Lengths     331/3         32  8/11      30  28  4/17       26  2/3       25 

Ratios  54/55      11/12      16/17      17/18        15/16 

Names         G°  G*  A-1  Bb  B_1         C° 

Cents         702  732  884  989         1088         1200 

M.D.  22.0;   S.D.  30.3 
Table  131.    Colonna's  Irregular  Just  Intonation,  No.  2 

Lengths  1920  2000        2160         2304         2400        2560         2688 

Ratios  24/25       25/27       15/16       24/25       15/16      20/21      14/15 

Names  C°  &*  "2         D°            Eb+1         E_1           F°            F* 

Cents            0  70  204          316          386          498          618 

Lengths  2880  3072         3200        3456         3520        3842 

Ratios  15/16        24/25      25/27      54/55      11/12 

Names  G°  Ab+1         A"1          Bb+1         B              C° 

Cents  702  814          884        1018         1048         1200 

M.D.  29.3;  S.D.  33.8 


155 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


possible  divisions  of  the  10:9  tone  are  13:12  and  40:39,  which  is 
somewhat  better  than  Colonna's  division,  and  the  linear  division 
19:18  and  20:19,  as  inGanassi.  Divisions  of  the  9:8  tone  include 
17:16  and  18:17,  as  well  as  15:14  and  21:20,  both  of  which  Co- 
lonna  used.  Other  possible  superparticular  divisions  of  the  9:8 
tone  are  13:12  and  27:26;  12:11  and  33:32;  11:10  and  45:44;  and 
10:9  and  81:80,  this  last,  of  course,  being  the  minor  tone  and 
comma. 

Divisions  of  the  Ditonic  Comma 

The  Pythagorean-type  temperaments  in  our  second  group  are 
more  difficult  to  construct,  in  that  they  contain  unusual  divisions 
of  the  ditonic  comma.  By  ear,  these  temperaments  would  have 
been  almost  impossible  in  many  cases,  because  there  are  no  pure 
intervals  to  check  by  as  in  some  varieties  of  the  meantone  tem- 
perament, nor  are  there  even  fairly  definite  tempered  intervals, 
such  as  the  C  E  G*  C  of  equal  temperament,  which  also  provide 
a  good  check.  For  the  division  of  the  monochord,  these  temper- 
aments could  have  been  set  down  readily  with  the  aid  of  loga- 
rithms, and  they  can  be  expressed  in  our  modern  cents  with  the 
greatest  of  ease.  Computers  who  did  not  use  logarithms  were 
able  to  achieve  comparable  results  by  a  linear  division  of  the 
comma,  but  had  less  success  if  they  ignored  the  schisma  which 
separates  the  syntonic  from  the  ditonic  comma.  In  most  of  our 
tables  we  shall  assume,  for  the  sake  of  convenience,  that  the  di- 
tonic comma  has  been  given  a  correct  geometric  division,  and 
shall  assign  cents  values  to  the  intervals  accordingly. 

The  leading  exponents  of  this  sophisticated  sort  of  comma- 
juggling  were  Werckmeister,  Neidhardt,  and  Marpurg.^4  Each 
has  expressed  the  alteration  of  his  fifths  and  thirds  in  the  12th 
part  of  a  comma,  which,  strictly,  should  be  the  ditonic  comma. 

^^See  Johann  George  Neidhardt,  Gantzlich  erschopfte,  mathematische  Ab- 
theilungen  des  diatonisch-chromatischen,  temperirten  Canonis  Monochordi 
(Konigsberg  and  Leipzig,  1732),  pp.  29  (the  Fifth-Circles)  and  38  (Third- 
Circles).  See  also  F.W.Marpurg,  Versuch  liber  die  musikalische  Temper- 
atur,  p.  158,  for  the  lettered  temperaments  A  through  L.  All  other  refer- 
ences will  be  indicated  in  footnotes. 

156 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


Since  the  ditonic  comma  is  approximately  24  cents,  this  means 
that  2  cents  will  be  taken  as  the  unit  of  tempering.  Thus  the  oc- 
tave would  contain  600  parts,  or  thereabouts.  This  is  an  inter- 
esting forerunner  of  the  cents  representation. 

In  evaluating  this  group  of  temperaments,  it  should  be  pointed 
out  that  there  are  two  opposing  points  of  view.  Since  we  are 
likely  to  regard  most  highly  those  irregular  systems  that  come 
closest  to  equal  temperament,  there  will  be  in  each  subclass  a 
temperament  by  Mar  pur  g  or  Neidhardt  that  wins  the  award  be- 
cause in  it  the  altered  fifths  are  symmetrically  arranged  among 
the  entire  12  fifths  of  the  temperament.  In  these  temperaments 
all  keys  are  pretty  much  alike,  whether  nearer  to  C  major  or  F* 
major. 

But  the  whole  intent  of  having  a  "circulating"  temperament, 
of  having  the  octave  "well  tempered,"  was  to  have  greater  con- 
sonance in  the  keys  most  used  than  in  those  more  remote.  This 
is  made  very  clear  in  the  writings  of  Werckmeister  and  Neid- 
hardt. We  should  fail  in  our  duty,  therefore,  did  we  not  refer  at 
the  end  of  this  chapter  to  temperaments  we  have  discussed  that 
satisfy  this  ideal  of  graduated  dissonance.  Both  Werckmeister 
and  Neidhardt  had  a  proper  respect  for  equal  temperament  also, 
but  a  fanatic  like  Tempelhof,  ^  writing  fifty  to  seventy-five  years 
later,  could  say  that  equal  temperament  was  the  worst  possible 
temperament  because  one  scale  must  differ  from  another  in  its 
tuning! 

The  simplest  alteration  of  the  Pythagorean  tuning  is  to  divide 
the  comma  into  two  equal  parts.  If  the  altered  fifths  are  consec- 
utive, there  will  be  a  temperament  somewhat  like  the  modifica- 
tion of  the  meantone  temperament  shown  at  the  beginning  of  this 
chapter.  This  is  Bamberger's  tuning,2**  except  that  he  has  di- 
vided the  syntonic  comma  arithmetically  between  the  fifths  D-A 
and  A-E,  thus  getting  a  slightly  smaller  deviation  than  if  he  had 
divided  the  ditonic  comma  (see  Table  132). 


2**Georg     Friedrich  Tempelhof,    Gedanken  iiber  die  Temperatur  des  Herrn 
Kirnberger  (Berlin  and  Leipzig,  1775),  pp.  10,  18. 

2°J.  P.  Kirnberger,  Die  Kunst  des  reinen  Satzes  in  der  Musik,  Part  I,  p.  13. 

157 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  132.    Kirnberger's  Temperament  (1/2-Comma) 


Ratios 

1       256:243 

9:8 

32:27 

5:4 

4:3 

45:32 

3:2 

128:81 

Names 

C° 

Dbo 

D° 

Eb° 

E"1 

F° 

F*-x 

G° 

Abo 

Cents 

0 

90 

204 

294 

386 

498 

590 

702 

792 

Ratios 

270:161     16:9 

15:8 

2:1 

Names 

ifl 

Bbo 

B-1 

C° 

Cents 

895 

996 

1088 

1200 

M.D.  9.0; 

S.D.  9 

7 

Baron  von  Wiese's  second  tuning  was  exactly  the  same  as 
Kirnberger's.  He  was  so  confirmed  a  Pythagorean  that  he  called 
E~\  F#"\  andB"1  by  the  respective  names  Fb°,  Gb°,  and  Cb°, 
each  of  which  would  have  been  2  cents  (the  schisma)  flatter  than 
the  corresponding  syntonic  value.  However,  von  Wiese's  first 
temperament^'  actually  dic^  divide  the  ditonic  comma,  making 
his  F*  the  mean  between  Db  and  B  (Table  133).  His  ratio  for 
F#,  5760:4073,  is  an  excellent  approximation  for  the  square  root 
of  one-half. 

Von  Wiese's  other  three  temperaments  are  respectable 
enough,  for  in  them  the  tempered  fifths  are  separated  by  a  minor 
or  major  third.  Since  the  deviation  is  the  same  for  all  three,  we 
show  No.  3  only  (Table  134).  Von  Wiese  has  indicated  it  as  ex- 
tending from  B"  to  D#;  but  from  the  construction  it  extends  from 
Gb  to  B,  with  the  fifths  Eb-Bb  and  B-Gb  each  tempered  by  half 
the  ditonic  comma.  The  best  arrangement  of  the  tempered  fifths 
is  for  them  to  be  separated  by  a  semitone  or  a  tritone.    The  lat- 

Table  133.  Von  Wiese's  Temperament,  No.  1  (1/2-Comma) 

Names    C°  Db°    D°      Eb°      E°      F°      F*~2   G°     Ab°     A°     Bb  °      B°        C° 
Cents       0    90     204     294     408    498    600     702    792    906    996     1110    1200 

M.D.  10.0;   S.D.  10.8 


^Christian  LudwigGustav,  Baron  von  Wiese,  Klangeintheilungs-,  Stimmungs- 
und  Temperatur-Lehre  (Dresden,  1793),  p.  9  (No.  1)  and  p.  12  (No.  3). 

158 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


Table  134.   Von  Wiese's  Temperament,  No.  3  (1/2-Comma) 

Names     C°  Db+5  D°      Eb+5  E°       F°     Gb+"2  G°     Ab+5   A0      Bb°     B°         C° 
Cents       0    102     204     306    408    498     600    702    804    906    996     1110     1200 

M.D.  5.0;   S.D.  6.6 

ter  arrangement  occurs  in  Grammateus'  temperament,  shown 
earlier  in  this  chapter,  which  is  identical  with  Marpurg's  K. 
Note  that  von  Wiese's  No.  3  is  the  same  as  Grammateus'  except 
for  Bb. 

Next  in  order  would  be  temperaments  in  which  the  ditonic 
comma  is  divided  among  three  thirds.  Charles,  Earl  Stanhope^o 
advocated  such  a  division,  but  indicated  that  the  syntonic  comma 
should  be  divided  among  the  fifths  G-D,  D-A,  and  A-E.  This  left 
the  schisma,  2  cents,  to  be  divided  among  the  four  fifths  from  Bb 
to  G*3,  the  other  five  fifths  being  pure.  Thus  the  four  black  keys 
are  only  one  cent  sharper  than  if  the  tuning  were  purely  Pytha- 
gorean. He  might  better  have  divided  the  ditonic  comma  among 
his  first  three  fifths,  and  not  have  had  the  approximate  fifths  to 
worry  over.  With  the  ditonic  comma  divided  among  three  con- 
secutive fifths,  the  mean  deviation  is  9.0,  the  standard  deviation 
9.7.  Stanhope's  own  temperament  (Table  135)  is  slightly  better 
than  this,  just  as  Kirnberger's  was  better  than  von  Wiese's  No. 
1,  because  the  former  divided  the  syntonic  comma. 

Werckmeister^  has  shown  a  temperament  in  which  the  comma 
is  divided  into  three  parts.  It  is,  however,  even  less  satisfactory 
than  Stanhope's,  because  it  contains  five  fifths  flat  by  1/3  comma, 
two  fifths  sharp  by  1/3  comma,  and  only  five  perfect  fifths  (see 
Table  136) .  This  is  the  poorest  of  the  three  temperaments  Werck- 
meister  called  "correct." 

Bendeler  has  used  the  1/3 -comma  tempering  in  two  of  his 


28"Principles  of  the  Science  of  Tuning  Instruments  with  Fixed  Tones,"  Philo- 
sophical Magazine,  XXV  (1806),  291-312. 

29Andreas  Werckmeister,  Musicalische  Temperatur  (Frankfort  and  Leipzig, 
1691),  Plate. 

159 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  135.    Stanhope's  Temperament  (1/3-Comma) 


Lengths 

120 

113.84 

107.1 

101.19 

96 

90 

Names 

C° 

Dbo 

i 

D"  3 

Ebo 

E"1 

Fu 

Cents 

0 

91 

197 

295 

386 

498 

Lengths 

80 

75.89 

71.7 

67.5 

64 

60 

Names 

G° 

Abo 

2 

A"" 

Bbu 

B-1 

C° 

Cents 

702 

793 

892 

996 

1088 

1200 

85.38 
nbo 


589 


M.D.  7.8;   S.D.  8.7 

three  organ  temperaments.^  In  the  first,  the  tempering  is  shared 
by  the  fifths  C-G,  G-D,  and  B-F*  (Table  137) .  Since  these  are 
not  all  consecutive  fifths  in  the  circle  of  fifths,  his  deviation  is 
considerably  less  than  Stanhope's. 

In  Bendeler's  second  temperament  (Table  138),  the  comma  is 
divided  among  the  three  fifths  C-G,  D-A,  and  F*-C#.    Since  the 

Table  136.    Werckmeister's  Correct  Temperament,  No.  2  (1/3-Comma) 


Names    C°  C#~3D 


ho 


f'-'g" 


,#■ 


.b+-i 


E-   e  3  F°   F"  'G3  G"  3A  3  B"'3   B    C° 
Cents   0  82  196  294  392  498  588  694  786  890  1004  1086  1200 

M.D.  9.2;  S.D.  10.7 


Table  137.    Bendeler's  Temperament,  No.  1  (1/3-Comma) 

J-i 


Eb°    E" 


F#_1  G" 


G#-iA-3    Bbo 


Names    C°  C"   *D   3     E""    E    3     F°       F'    *  G  3    G"    *A   3    B"u      B  3      C° 
Cents       0    90     188     294     392     498     588     694     792    890    996     1094     1200 

M.D.  5.0;  S.D.  5.8 


Table  138.    Bendeler's  Temperament,  No.  2  (1/3-Comma) 

bo 


Names    C°  C^D"3    E*30    E 


F°     Fff   3  G 


G        A 


B"        B    3     C° 

Cents       0    90     196     294     392     498     596     694     792    890    996     1094     1200 

M.D.  4.0;   S.D.  4.8 


30 


J.  P.  Bendeler,  Organopoeia  (2nd  ed.;    Frankfurt  and  Leipzig,  1739),  p.  40 
(No.  1)  and  p.  42  (No.  2). 

160 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


fifths  are  more  widely  separated  than  before,  the  deviation  is 
less  than  for  No.  1. 

The  best  arrangement  of  the  three  tempered  fifths  is  to  have 
them  separated  by  major  thirds,  as  in  Marpurg's  I,  where  E  and 
G*  are  the  same  pitches  as  in  equal  temperament  (see  Table  139). 

The  most  famous  of  Werckmeister's  irregular  divisions  has 
the  comma  divided  equally  among  the  four  fifths  C-G,  G-D,  D-A, 
and  B-F#.31  since  three  of  these  fifths  are  consecutive,  the  de- 
viation is  comparatively  large  (see  Table  140).  This  is  the  only 
temperament  that  Sorge  has  ascribed  to  Werckmeister.  The 
same  division  was  accepted  by  Marpurg,  and  a  modern  acousti- 
cian, Karl  Erich  Schumann, **  has  followed  suit,  without  men- 
tioning any  secondary  source. 

In  Werckmeister's  third  "correct"  temperament  (Table  141), 
five  fifths  (D-A,  A-E,  F#-C#,  C#-G#,  and  F-C)  are  flattened  by 
1/4  comma,  and  one  fifth,  G*-D#,  is  raised  by  the  same  amount. 
Thanks,  however,  to  the  more  nearly  symmetrical  arrangement 
of  the  tempered  fifths,  the  deviation  is  slightly  less  than  for  his 
first  temperamento 

In  his  third  temperament,  Bendeler,^^  unhampered  by  a  sharp 
fifth  and  with  a  fairly  symmetrical  arrangement  of  the  four  flat- 
tened fifths  (C-G,  G-D,  E-B,  G*-D#),  succeeded  in  achieving  a 
very  good  division  (Table  142). 

But,  as  usual,  the  best  temperament  for  a  particular  division 
of  the  comma  is  completely  symmetrical,  and  so  Neidhardt,  in 
his  fourth  Fifth-Circle  (Table  143),  gave  Eb,  F#,  and  A  the  same 
pitches  they  would  have  in  equal  temperament.  (Marpurg's  H  is 
identical  with  this.) 

When  the  comma  is  divided  into  five  parts  and  the  tempered 
fifths  are  arranged  as  symmetrically  as  possible,  the  deviation 
begins  to  approach  the  vanishing  point.  (Paradoxically,  this  de- 
viation is  lower  than  for  a  wholly  symmetrical  arrangement  of 
six  fifths  tempered  by  1/6  comma,  shown  in  the  next  section.)    In 


31Werckmeister  (see  Table  1-iO),  loc.  cit. 
32Akustik  (Breslau,  1925),  p.  31. 
33Organopoeia,  p.  42. 


161 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Table  139.    Marpurg's  Temperament  I  (1/3-Comma) 

Names   C°  Cf^D°      E°+*   E~3  F+3    F*~3  G°      G#"  A0       Bb+3     B"5     C° 
Cents      0    106    204     302    400     506    604     702    800    906    1004     1102     1200 

M.D.  3.0;   S.D.  3.5 


Table  140.    Werckmeister's  Correct  Temperament,  No.  1  (1/4-Comma) 

Names  C°    C#_1D~2    £b°     E~4     F°       F^G'4"    G#~l  A~4   Bb°      B~4       C° 
Cents       0    90     192     294     390     498     588     696    792     888    996     1092     1200 

M.D.  6.0;   S.D.  7.5 


Table  141.    Werckmeister's  Correct  Temperament,  No.  3  (1/4-Comma) 

Names    C°  C^D0      E     4  E   2    F   *     ¥w   2G°      Gff      A  4      B      4    B    2      C° 
Cents       0    96    204     300    396     504     600    702    792    900    1002     1098     1200 

M.D.  5.0;  S.D.  5.7 


Table  142.    Bendeler's  Temperament,  No.  3  (1/4-Comma) 

Names    C°    Cff   *D   2  E        E    2     F°     Fff'  4  G   4    G*    4  A    2    Bu       B~4       C° 
Cents       0    96     192    294     396    498     594     696    798     894    996     1092     1200 

M.D.  3.3;  S.D.  3.7 


Table  143.    Neidhardt's  Fifth-Circle,  No.  4  (1/4-Comma) 

U-2     _J        h+-1       -i  #--!       -i         H-2       _i         ho  -i 

Names    C°  C      4D   4    E      4E    2     F°      F      2G   4    G     4A   4    B         B    2      C° 
Cents       0    96     198     300     396    498     600     696    798     900    996     1098     1200 

M.D.  2.7;   S.D.  2.8 


Table  144.    Marpurg's  Temperament  G  (1/5- Comma) 

Names  C°  Cff    5D    5    E      5E    5     F°      F*    5  G°     G *   *  A" 5    B      5     B~~5      C° 
Cents      0    100    199    299    398    498    602    702    802    901     1001     1100    1200 

M.D.    .7;   S.D.  1.3 

162 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


Marpurg'sG  (Table  144)  this  near-symmetrical  division  is  made. 
Marpurg  called  the  amount  of  tempering  2^-/12  =  5/24  comma, 
which  would  be  5  cents,  slightly  larger  than  1/5  comma  or  4.8 
cents.  Although  the  difference  between  the  two  is  wholly  negli- 
gible, the  latter  amount  of  tempering  has  been  used  in  making 
the  table,  with  the  values  rounded  off  to  even  cents. 

The  1/6-comma  temperament  is  recommended  by  Thomas 
Young,  34  as  a  simpler  method  than  the  irregular  temperament 
described  later  in  this  chapter.  In  his  own  words,  "In  practice, 
nearly  the  same  effect  may  be  very  simply  produced,  by  tuning 
C  to  F,  Bb,  Eb,  G#,  C#,  F#  six  perfect  fourths;  and  C,  G,  D,  A, 
E,  B,  F#  six  equally  imperfect  fifths."  In  other  words,  he  had 
six  consecutive  fifths  tempered  by  l/6ditonic  comma  (see  Table 
145).   As  a  practical  tuning  method,  this  would  not  be  difficult, 

Table  145.    Young's  Temperament  No.  2  (1/6-Comma) 

Names    C°  Dbo   D_3    Ebo    E~3     F°      Gbo      G"*  Ab°    A~2    Bb°       B~«     C° 
Cents       0    90     196     294     392    498     588    698     792    894    996     1090     1200 

M.D.  6.0;   S.D.  6.8 

and  it  certainly  does  differentiate  between  near  and  remote  keys. 
This  is  the  tuning  of  the  Out-Of-Tune  Piano,  the  sort  of  tuning 
into  which  a  piano  originally  in  equal  temperament  might  fall  if 
played  upon  by  a  beginner  .35  Young's  key  of  G  is  the  best,  that 
of  Db  the  worst.  If  he  had  commenced  his  set  of  tempered  fifths 
with  F  instead  of  C,  the  key  of  C  would  have  been  best. 

In  Neidhardt's  second  Fifth-Circle  (Table  146),  all  the  fifths 

Table  146.    Neidhardt's  Fifth-Circle,  No.  2  (1/6-Comma) 

Names    C°  Cff    2D°     E      6  E    3    F   «    F      3G~?  G      3A""e     B      3    B  "2    C° 
Cents      0     102     204    298    400    502    604     698    800    902     1004     1098    1200 

M.D.  3.0;   S.D.  3.4 

34"  Outlines  of  Experiments  and  Inquiries  Respecting  Sound  and  Light,"  Philo- 
sophical Transactions,  XC  (1800),  145. 

35j.  Murray  Barbour,  "Bach  and  The  Art  of  Temperament,"    Musical  Quar- 
terly, XXXm  (1947),  66  f,  89. 

163 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


are  altered  by  1/6  comma,  nine  being  lowered  and  three  raised. 
Since  the  arrangement  is  completely  symmetrical,  the  deviation 
is  low. 

Of  course,  a  symmetrical  arrangement  of  fifths  alternately 
pure  and  lowered  by  1/6  comma  comes  closest  to  equal  temper- 
ament. Both  Neidhardt  (Third  Fifth-Circle)  and  Marpurg  (F) 
have  presented  this  temperament  (Table  147).  Observe  that  in 
it  the  consecutive  notes  are  alternately  the  same  as  in  equal  tem- 
perament and  2  cents  higher,  so  that  the  mean  deviation  and 
standard  deviation  both  are  equal  to  2.0.  More  elaborate  patterns 
of  semitones  either  2  cents  higher  or  lower  than  in  equal  tem- 
perament could  be  obtained  by  having  two  pure  fifths  alternate 

Table  147.    Neidhardt's  Fifth-Circle,  No.  3  (1/6-Comma) 

a-i     --1        h+i      --1        +-1       u-1  u-2     -1        hj-  '        -J 

Names  C°  C*    2D    6    E      3E    3    F    6   F      2G°       Gff    3A    6     B      6    B    3     C° 

Cents      0     102     200    302    400    502     600    702    800    902     1000    1102    1200 

M.D.  2.0;   S.D.  2.0 

with  two  tempered  fifths,  or  by  having  three  pure  fifths  similarly 
alternate  with  three  tempered  ones. 

Bermudo,36  wno  had  also  formed  equal  semitones  on  the  lute 
by  the  method  of  Grammateus,  made  a  real  contribution  to  tuning 
theory  in  a  chapter  "concerning  the  seven-stringed  vihuela  upon 
which  all  the  semitones  can  be  played."  This  was  a  method  in- 
tended for  experienced  players.  His  account  of  the  division  is 
necessarily  lengthy  and  need  not  be  given  as  a  whole.  G  is  the 
fundamental,  and  there  are  10  frets,  thus  making  no  provision 
for  F#  on  this  string.  The  notes  from  E^  to  G  inclusive  are 
formed  by  a  succession  of  pure  fifths.  The  thirds  G-B  and  A-C* 
are  each  2/3  syntonic  comma  sharper  than  pure  thirds.  The  tone 
G-A  is  1/6  comma  less  than  a  major  tone.  Then  D  and  E  form  pure 
fourths  with  A  and  B,  respectively,  and  G*  is  a  fourth  below  C*. 

The  geometry,  which  consists  of  linear  divisions  only,  is  easy 
to  follow,  especially  with  the  aid  of  Bermudo's  monochord  dia- 


3" J.  Bermudo,  Declaracion  de  instrumentos  musicales  (Ossuna,  1555),  Book 
4,  Chap.  86. 

164 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


gram  (see  Figure  I).    In  ratios,  as  will  be  seen  in  Table  148,  it 
becomes  quite  complicated,  and,  if  these  ratios  were  to  be  rep- 

r  a  b         C  D  b      f  c 

b 1 v 1 ■ — !-■-, \ -i-H 1 — i — i i : ; , 


Fig.  I.    Bermudo's  Method  for  Placing  Frets  on  the  Vihuela 

Reproduced  by  courtesy  of  the  Library  of  Congress 


Table  148.    Bermudo's  Vihuela  Temperament  (1/6-  1/2-Comma) 


Names 

G° 

G#" 

A" 

Bb° 

B" 

c° 

Ratios 

1 

492075:463684 

540:481 

32:27 

1215:964 

4:3 

Cents 

0 

102.9 

200.3 

294.2 

400.6 

498.0 

Names 

C# 

i                         i 

2                               D"6 

Eb° 

i 

F° 

k 

i 

G° 

Ratios  164025:115921  720:481  128:81  405:241  16:9  [218700:11592l]  2:1 
Cents  600.9  698.3         792.1       898.6      996.1  [1098.9J  1200 

M.D.  3.9;   S.D.  4.2 

resented  by  least  integers,  as  was  done  in  many  of  these  systems, 
the  fundamental  note  G  would  have  to  be  62,985,600!  Let  us  as- 
sume that  F*,  the  unused  11th  fret,  is  a  pure  fourth  above  C*. 

The  reason  Bermudo's  system  is  presented  in  connection  with 
the  use  of  fifths  tempered  by  1/6  comma  is  that  that  is  precisely 
what  he  has.  If  the  temperament  of  successive  fifths  is  examined, 
it  will  be  seen  that  the  fifths  on  G,  A,  and  B  are  each  tempered 
by  1/6  comma,  eight  fifths  are  pure,  and  the  usual  wolf  fifth,  G#- 
E",  is  1/2  comma  flat.  (It  really  should  not  be  called  a  wolf  fifth, 
since  it  is  flat,  not  sharp,  and  the  usual  poor  thirds  of  the  mean- 
tone  temperaments,  on  B  through  G#,  are  the  best  of  all!) 

This  is  the  first  time,  so  far  as  is  known,  that  any  writer  had 
suggested  the  formation  of  notes  used  in  equal  temperament  by 
the  proper  division  of  the  comma  for  those  notes.  Of  course  he 
was  making  an  arithmetical  division  of  the  syntonic  comma,  and 
thus  had  small  errors.  But  so  did  the  late  seventeenth  and  most 
of  the  eighteenth  century  comma-splitters  from  Werckmeister 
to  Kirnberger  and  Stanhope.    Bermudo's  three  tempered  fifths 

165 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


are  as  symmetrically  arranged  as  in  the  Neidhardt-Marpurg  sys- 
tem shown  before  this.  It  is  too  bad  he  did  not  continue  his  proc- 
ess by  tempering  D#  by  2/3  comma  and  E#  by  5/6  comma.  Then 
he  would  not  have  had  the  half-comma  error  concentrated  on  a 
single  fifth,  nor  a  Pythagorean  third  on  E^.  But  this  method  of 
Bermudo  is  worthy  of  our  respect  as  a  very  early  approach  to 
equal  temperament,  somewhat  difficult,  but  not  impracticable  for 
a  skilled  performer  to  use. 

Werckmeister  is  the  only  later  writer  to  temper  his  fifths  by 
the  7th  part  of  a  comma,  perhaps  following  the  example  of  Zar- 
lino's  2/7-comma  variety  of  meantone  temperament.^'  But  his 
Septenarium  temperament  is  a  rather  eccentric  thing.  In  it  the 
fifths  C-G,  BO-F,  and  B-F#  are  1/7  comma  flat;  F#-C#  is  2/7 
comma  flat;  G-D  is  4/7  comma  flat;  D-A  and  G*-D*  are  1/7 
comma  sharp;  the  remaining  five  fifths  are  pure.    (The  cents 

Table  149.    Werckmeister's  Septenarium  Temperament  (1/7-Comma) 
Lengths     196    186    176    165    156    147    139    131    124    117     110      104       98 

5  14  11      5  X        it  4  t-»  4 

Names        C°     Cri  D~7  ED+"7  E_"7    F°     F#_7G~7  G       A~~7    B      7    B~7      C° 
Cents  0       91     186    298    395    498    595    698    793    893    1000    1097    1200 

M.D.  4.7;   S.D.  5.6 

Table  150.    Symmetric  Septenarium  Temperament  (1/7-Comma) 

M-4        _J  h+  2         _2  +J  U      4        _  1  U       5        _2  h,J  3 

Names    C°    C"    7D    7  E      7E   "7    F    7   Fff""7G    7    Gff_7A    7    B      7    B" 7      C° 
Cents       0     100    201     301     401     501     598     699     799     899    999     1100    1200 

M.D.  0.5;   S.D.  1.0 

values  have  been  worked  out  from  Werckmeister' s  string-lengths , 
and  are  slightly  inaccurate.) 

For  the  sake  of  a  comparison  with  Werckmeister's  temper- 
ament, a  symmetric  version  of  the  1/7 -comma  temperament  is 
shown  in  Table  150.  It  is  even  nearer  equal  temperament  than 
Marpurg'sG,  which  had  a  symmetric  distribution  of  the  fifth  part 
of  the  comma. 

3'A.  Werckmeister,  Musicalische  Temperatur,  Plate. 
166 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


Next  we  have  a  large  group  of  temperaments  in  which  some 
fifths  are  tempered  by  1/5  comma  and  others  by  1/12  comma, 
while  the  remaining  fifths  are  pure.  Since  1/12  comma  is  the 
temperament  of  the  fifth  of  equal  temperament,  there  will  be  as 
many  pure  fifths  as  there  are  fifths  tempered  by  1/5  comma. 
This  group  of  temperaments  might  be  considered,  therefore,  as 
variants  of  the  previously  described  temperaments  in  which  there 
are  six  pure  fifths  and  six  fifths  tempered  by  1/6  comma., 

Neidhardt  was  the  great  inventor  of  temperaments  in  which 
the  comma  was  divided  into  both  6  parts  and  12  parts. 38  All 
three  "circulating"  temperaments  fall  into  this  group.  They  hap- 
pen to  be  among  the  poorest  ofthis  type  that  he  or  the  other  the- 
orists have  evolved  — that  is,  when  compared  with  equal  temper- 
ament. But  we  shall  see  that  they  do  satisfy  Neidhardt' s  purpose 
in  creating  them.  The  first  circulating  temperament  (Table  151) 
has  four  fifths  in  each  group  —  pure,  tempered  by  1/12  comma, 
and  by  1/6  comma.  Since  four  consecutive  fifths  in  it  are  tem- 
pered by  1/6  comma,  it  may  be  considered  a  variant  of  the  1/5- 
comma  meantone  temperament. 

The  first  of  Thomas  Young's  pair  of  temperaments  is  very 
like  the  Neidhardt  temperament  shown  in  Table  151. ^  Young 
said,  "It  appears  to  me,  that  every  purpose  maybe  answered,  by 
making  C:E  too  sharp  by  a  quarter  of  a  comma,  which  will  not 
offend  the  nicest  ear;  E:G#  and  At>:C  equal;  F*:A*  too  sharp  by 
a  comma;  and  the  major  thirds  of  all  the  intermediate  keys  more 
or  less  perfect  as  they  approach  more  or  less  to  C  in  the  order 
of  modulation." 

Table  151.    Neidhardt's  Circulating  Temperament,  No.  1 
(1/12-,  1/6-Comma) 

_  J       h°  -  -3 

A    2  B  B    4      C° 

894    996     1092     1200 


mes    C°  C#""6D~"3  Eb° 

--                    #--5      --1        U- 
E    a    F°       Fff    6  G    6    G* 

nts       0    94     196     296 

392    498     592     698     796 

M.D.  4.0;  S.D.  4.6 

"*°J.  G.  Neidhardt,  Sectio  canonis  harmonici,  pp.  16-18. 
39Thomas  Young,  in  Philosophical  Transactions,  XC  (1800),  145  f. 


167 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Young  accomplished  the  first  result  by  tempering  the  fifths 
on  C,  G,  D,  and  A  by  3/16  syntonic  comma,  and  the  other  results 
by  tempering  the  fifths  on  F,  B",  E,  and  B  by  approximately  1/12 
syntonic  comma,  and  leaving  the  other  four  fifths  pure.  The  total 
amount  of  tempering  would  be  13/12  syntonic  comma,  this  being 
sufficiently  close  to  the  ratio  of  the  ditonic  to  the  syntonic  comma. 
Young  has  given  numbers  for  his  monochord,  and  they  agree  well 
with  his  theory.  He  has  made  a  mistake,  however,  in  calculating 
the  length  for  ED  (83810),  which  was  intended  as  a  pure  fourth  be- 
low gC    The  corrected  length  is  given  in  Table  152. 


Table 

152.    Young's  Temperament 

No.  1 

(1/12- ,  3/1C 

-Comma 

) 

Lengths 

Names 

100000 
C° 

94723 

C       12 

89304 

3 
D      « 

84197 

79752 

3 

e""4 

74921 

i 

Y+  i2 

71041 

#-- 
Y      i2 

Cents 

0 

94 

196 

298 

392 

500 

592 

Lengths 
Names 

66822 

3 

G    16 

63148 
G      12 

59676 

9 

A"16 

56131 

53224 

5 
B"6 

50000 
C° 

Cents 

698 

796 

894 

1000 

1092 

1200 

M.D.  5.3;   S.D.  5.9 

Now  3/16  syntonic  comma  is  an  awkward  interval  to  deal  with. 
If,  instead,  we  take  1/6  ditonic  comma  as  the  temperament  of 
Young's  four  diatonic  fifths,  and  1/12  ditonic  comma  for  his  sec- 
ond group  of  fifths,  his  monochord  will  be  precisely  of  theNeid- 
hardt  type.  The  differences  from  the  monochord  he  did  give  are 
so  small  that  the  cents  values  do  not  differ.  The  arrangement  of 
his  second  group  of  fifths  is  slightly  different  from  Neidhardt's, 
and  this  accounts  for  the  difference  in  deviation. 

Mercadier's  temperament  (Table  153)  closely  resembles 
Young's,  even  to  the  total  amount  of  tempering  —  13/12  syntonic 
comma. ^  He  directed  that  the  fifths  from  C  to  E  should  be  flat 
by  1/6  syntonic  comma,  and  those  from  E  to  G*  flat  by  1/12  comma. 
Then  G#  is  taken  as  AD,  the  next  three  fifths  are  to  be  just,  and 
the  fifth  F-C  then  turns  out  to  be  about  1/12  comma  flat. 


40Antoine    Suremain-Missery,     Theorie    acoustico-musicale     (Paris,    1793), 
p.  256. 


168 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


Table  153.    Mercadier's  Temperament  (1/12-,  1/6-Comma) 

Names    C°C*  12D    3  E      12E    3  F    12  Fff    6  G   6  G*    J  A  "2    B      12B~4       C° 
Cents       0    94     197     296    394     500    594     698     794     895    998     1094     1200 

M.D.  4.1;   S.D.  4.5 


Table  154.    Marpurg's  Temperament  D  (1/12-,  1/6-Comma) 

#_  2      _i        h+i        --£-  '{-  J  !         #     3  1  l,     l  1 

Names    C°    C      3D    4E      4E    12    F°     F      2  G   8  G     4A~4    B     ~2B"       C° 
Cents       0    98     198    300     398    498     600     698     798    900    998     1098     1200 

M.D.  1.3;   S.D.  1.6 


Table  155.    Neidhardt's  Circulating  Temperament,  No.  2 
(1/12-,  1/6-Comma) 

u-2      _i       h+-      --       +-       #-^      --i         ¥--      --  b+-         -- 

Names  C°  Cff   4D    3  E      6E    12  F    12  F      3G   6   G      6A    2    B      6    B    12      C° 
Cents       0    96     196    298     394     500    596    698    796    894     1000    1096     1200 

M.D.  3.3;  S.D.  3.7 


Table  156.    Neidhardt's  Circulating  Temperament,  No.  3 
(1/12-,  1/6-Comma) 

#_3     _i      h+-      -—  #--2      --1        #--5      --1      b+-i-      -- 

Names    C°  C      4D    3E      6E    12  F°       Fff    3  G   4    Gff    6  A    2  B      12  B    12     C° 

Cents       0    96     196    298     394    498     596     696    796    894    998     1096     1200 

M.D.  2.7;  S.D.  2.9 


Table  157.    Neidhardt's  Third-Circle,  No.  4  (1/12-,  1/6-Comma) 

Names    C°  Cff    4D    3  E        E    2    F°      Fff    3G   6    Gff   6A   2    B      6    B    3      C° 
Cents       0    96     196    296    396    498     596    698     796    894     1000    1094     1200, 

M.D.  2.7;  S.D.  3.4 


169 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


As  usual,  Marpurg  has  presented  the  symmetric  version  (Ta- 
ble 154)  of  the  above  temperaments.    It  has  negligible  deviations. 

In  the  second  and  third  of  Neidhardt's  "circulating"  temper- 
aments, six  fifths  are  tempered  by  1/12  comma,  and  three  each 
are  pure  or  are  tempered  by  1/6  comma.  These  two  tempera- 
ments (Tables  155  and  156)  are  quite  similar,  both  containing 
three  consecutive  fifths  tempered  by  1/6  comma.  Thus  they  pos- 
sibly represent  the  extreme  case  of  modification  of  the  1/6-comma 
meantone  temperament.  Number  3  has  a  shade  greater  sym- 
metry and  hence  smaller  deviation. 

Temperaments  4  and  3  of  Neidhardt's  Third-Circle  have  de- 
viations very  similar  to  those  of  the  temperaments  shown  in  Ta- 
bles 155  and  156.  In  fact,  their  mean  deviations  are  equal  re- 
spectively to  those  of  No.  2  and  No.  3  in  these  tables,  but  their 
standard  deviations  are  higher  because  they  contain  some  sharp 
fifths.  In  No.  4  (Table  157),  there  are  three  fifths  tempered  by 
1/12  comma  and  five  by  1/6  comma;  three  fifths  are  pure,  and 
one  is  1/12  comma  sharp.  In  No.  3  (Table  158),  four  fifths  are 
1/12  comma  flat,  six  are  1/6  comma  flat,  and  two  are  1/6  comma 
sharp.  (The  same  tempered  fifths  as  in  No.  3  appear  in  our  hy- 
pothetical version  of  Schlick's  temperament,  but  differently  ar- 
ranged.) 

Once  again  Marpurg  has  given  the  symmetric  version  of  Neid- 
hardt's temperaments,  specifically  of  the  second  and  third  "cir- 
culating" temperaments. 

Logically  we  show  next  two  temperaments  (Tables  160  and 
161)  in  which  eight  fifths  are  flat  by  1/12  comma  and  two  by  1/6 
comma,  while  two  are  pure.  Such  a  temperament  is  the  fifth  of 
Neidhardt's  Third-Circle. 

The  temperament  shown  in  Table  160  comes  so  close  to  equal 
temperament  that  in  practice  it  could  not  be  improved  upon.  But 
the  canny  Marpurg  has  halved  its  deviation  by  using  greater  sym- 
metry (see  Table  161). 

Another  temperament  of  Neidhardt  has  the  same  deviations  as 
those  of  his  fifth  Third-Circle  (Table  160).  This  is  the  fifth  tem- 
perament in  his  Fifth-Circle  (Table  162),  in  which  six  fifths  are 


170 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


Table  158.    Neidhardt's  Third-Circle,  No.  3  (1/12-,  1/6-Comma) 

Names    C°  C"*D""^  E^     e"^¥+T2  ¥f~T2G~~6  G*""6A"T2  Bb+*     b"      C° 
Cents       0    96     196    296    394     500    598     698    796    896     1002     1092     1200 

MD.  3.3;  S.D.  4.7 


Table  159.    Marpurg's  Temperament  C  (1/12-,  1/6-Comma) 
Names  C    C#"3D--s  Ebn  E"i  Fo       FMG-*GHA-*     Bb+°  B~&      C° 
Cents       0    98     200    300    400    498     600    700    800    898     1000    1100    1200 

M.D.  1.0;  S.D.  1.4 

Table  160.    Neidhardt's  Third-Circle,  No.  5  (1/12-,  1/6-Comma) 

U     —         1       h+  1  —      4-  1         U-—     -—      U     2      --         ho-1         -1 

Names    C°    C  ff~12D~«  E      *  E    »F   •    Fff    12G    12Gff~~3A   4    B     "    B_  2      C° 
Cents       0     100    200    300    398     502     598    700    800    900    1000    1098    1200 

M.D.  1.3;  S.D.  2.0 

Table  161.    Marpurg's  Temperament  B  (1/12- ,  1/6-Comma) 

■#      2  i  hx1  1  -uJ-        #-i       -i  #-.3        -i  h  +  i  -  — 

Names  C°  C      3D~4    E° +4  E    3    F+12Fff    2G   6    G*    4A    3    B°    6    B    12    C° 
Cents       0    98    198    298    400    500    600    698     798    898     1000    1100    1200 

M.D.    .7;   S.D.  1.1 

Table  162.    Neidhardt's  Fifth-Circle,  No.  5  (1/12- ,  1/6-Comma) 

Names    C°  Cff    12D    6  E      6E    *   F    6    F*    2G    12G#    3A    3    B     4    B~3      C° 
Cents      0    100    200    298    402     502    600    700    800    898     1002    1102    1200 

M.D.  1.3;  S.D.  2.0 


171 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


flat  by  1/12  comma  and  four  by  1/6  comma,  while  two  are  sharp 
by  1/12  comma. 

The  remaining  temperaments  in  this  group  come  from  Mar- 
purg.  The  first  (Table  163)  of  his  temperaments  in  which  some 
fifths  are  sharp  contains  six  fifths  flat  by  1/6  comma,  and  three 
fifths  each  flat  or  sharp  by  1/12  comma. ***■  Obviously,  this  is  a 
variant  upon  the  temperament  in  which  six  fifths  are  flat  by  1/6 
comma,  the  other  six  pure.  The  mean  deviation,  2.0,  is  the 
same,  but,  as  expected,  the  standard  deviation  is  higher  here. 
Other  possible  variants  would  contain,  in  addition  to  the  six  fifths 
tempered  by  1/6  comma,  two  fifths  each  flat  or  sharp  by  1/12 
comma  or  pure;  or  four  pure  fifths  and  one  each  flat  or  sharp 
by  1/12  comma. 

The  second  temperament  (Table  164)  in  this  other  set  byMar- 
purg  has  fifths  that  do  not  differ  greatly  from  those  in  the  pre- 
vious temperament.  Here  the  six  fifths  are  tempered  by  the  un- 
usual amount  of  5/24  comma  (shown  as  the  same  fraction  that 
did  duty  as  1/5  in  his  Temperament  G,  but  really  5/24  this  time), 
and  three  each  are  pure  or  1/12  comma  sharp. 

In  Marpurg's  Temperament  A  (Table  165),  ten  fifths  are  flat 
by  1/12  comma,  and  one  each  is  pure  or  1/6  comma  flat.  This  is 
as  far  as  one  can  go  in  this  direction,  for  the  next  step  would  be 
to  have  twelve  fifths  flat  by  1/12  comma  —  that  is,  equal  tem- 
perament. 

The  other  limit  for  this  sequence  of  temperaments  by  Mar- 
purg  is  his  own  Temperament  F,  already  shown  as  Neidhardt's 
Fifth-Circle,  No.  3  (Table  147).  In  it  there  are  no  fifths  tem- 
pered by  1/12  comma,  and  six  fifths  each  pure  or  flat  by  1/6 
comma.  Just  before  it  in  the  set  comes  Temperament  E  (Ta- 
ble 166),  which  has  two  fifths  flat  by  1/12  comma,  and  five  fifths 
each  pure  or  flat  by  1/6  comma. 

Marpurg's  Temperament  E,  shown  in  Table  166,  has  the  least 
deviation  of  the  five  temperaments  in  the  set.  Note  the  devia- 
tions again:  A,  1.7,1.8;  B,  0.7,1.1;  C,  1.0, 1.4;  D,  1.3, 1.6;  E, 
0.3,0.8.  From  the  table  for  E  it  is  easy  to  see  why  its  deviation 
is  low:    there  are  seven  consecutive  notes  with  cents  values  end- 

^Marpurg,  Versuch  liber  die  musikalische  Temperatur,  p.  163. 
172 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


Table  163.    Marpurg's  Temperament,  No.  1  (1/12- ,  1/6-Comma) 

Names    C°  C      2D    12  E      12E    3  F    6    F      12G    12G      3A    6    B     4    B   4     C° 
Cents       0    102    202    304    400    502    602    704    800    902    1002    1104    1200 

M.D.  2.0;  S.D.  2.4 


Table  164.    Marpurg's  Temperament,  No.  2  (1/12-,  5/24-Comma) 

a_3       _±       b+i         _i  __1         #_3        __5         #_2        __5.         b_JL        _i? 

Names  C°    C#    4D    12E      8  E    3    F    12  Fw    4G    24  G      3A    12  B      12B    24     C° 
Cents       0    96     194     297     400    496    594     697    800    896    994     1097     1200 

M.D.  3.0;  S.D.  3.1 


Table  165.    Marpurg's  Temperament  A  (1/12- ,  1/6-Comma) 

Names  C°    C#    2  D   6    E      4E    4    F    12  Fff    12G    12Gff    12A    6    B      •    B    3    C° 
Cents       0    102    200    300    402     500    602    700    802    902    1000    1102    1200 

M.D.  1.7;   S.D.  1.8 

Table  166.    Marpurg's  Temperament  E  (1/12-,  1/6-Comma) 

Names    C°    C#_T2D"  12Eb+-5E~4    F+1    F#"^G_^G#~ 3  A-^    B°+*  B_"     C° 
Cents      0    100    202    302    402    502    602    700    800    900   1000    1100    1200 

M.D.  0.3;  S.n0.8 

Table  167.    Neidhardt's  Fifth-Circle,  No.  6  (1/12-,  1/4-Comma) 

M-—      _J  K+i       --  i       U  -i       -_L       #5,1  hj^  1  7 

Names    C°  Cff    12D    3    E     4E    3    F    12  F     2G    12G     6A    4    B      6    B-"    C° 
Cents      0    100    196    300    400    496    600    700    796    900   1000    1096    1200 

M.D.  2.7;  S.D.  3.3 

Table  168.    Neidhardt's  Fifth-Circle,  No.  9  (1/12-,  1/4-Comma) 

ji.i    _i      h+i       -i  ik    2        J.      a    2        >       hn  ± 

Names    C°  C#    ^    3E      4    E    3    F°      Fff    3  G    12  G      3A"3    B        B" 12     C° 

Cents      0    98     196    300     400    498    596     700    800    898    996    1100    1200 

M.D.  2.0;  S.D.  2.4 

173 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


ing  in  00,  and  five  ending  in  02.  Therefore  the  total  deviation 
will  be  only  4  cents,  or  a  mean  deviation  of  0.3.  In  the  other 
temperaments  of  the  set,  some  values  end  in  00  and  others  in  98 
or  02.  But  in  no  other  temperament  do  all  the  00' s  come  together 
as  they  do  in  E.  Therefore  the  deviation  is  higher  in  the  other 
temperamentSo  But  it  need  not  have  been  higher.  If  in  A  the  pure 
fifth  is  followed  directly  by  the  fifth  flat  by  1/6  comma,  there 
will  be  only  one  note  with  an  02  ending,  and  eleven  notes  with  00. 
The  fifths  in  B,  C,  and  D  can  be  so  arranged  that  there  will  be 
respectively  2,  3,  and  4  consecutive  notes  with  an  02  (or  98)  end- 
ing, the  other  endings  being  00.  Thus  the  minimum  deviation 
(M.D.  0.3;  S.D.  0.8)  will  be  the  same  for  all  five  temperaments, 
but  this  will  not  always  involve  the  most  symmetrical  version  of 
the  fifths. 

The  remaining  nine  temperaments  are  all  by  Neidhardt,  and 
each  contains  some  fifths  tempered  by  1/4  comma.  His  Fifth- 
Circle,  No.  6  (Table  167)  has  four  fifths  each  flat  by  1/4  comma 
or  flat  or  sharp  by  1/12  comma.    His  arrangement  is  symmetric. 

In  Temperament  No.  9  of  this  same  set  (Table  168),  Neidhardt 
has  three  fifths  flat  by  1/4  comma,  three  flat  by  1/12  comma,  and 
six  pure.  Again  the  arrangement  is  symmetric.  The  deviation 
is  lower  than  for  the  previous  temperament. 

In  Temperaments  7  and  10  (Table  169  and  170),  Neidhardt  di- 
vides the  comma  into  4  or  6  parts.  No.  7  is  especially  compli- 
cated, having  eight  fifths  flat  by  1/6  comma  and  two  sharp  by  1/6 
comma,  and  one  each  flat  or  sharp  by  1/4  comma.  It  would  be 
difficult  to  construct  a  symmetric  arrangement  from  such  an  ar- 
ray, and  Neidhardt  has  not  attempted  to  do  so. 

Table  169.    Neidhardt's  Fifth-Circle,  No.  7  (1/6-,  1/4-Comma) 

Names  C°  C#    6D    12E      6  E    3    F    6    F*    3  G   4    G*    3A    12B  3    2      C° 

Cents      0    94     194    298     400    494    596     696    800    892    996    1098    1200 

M.D.  3.3;  S.D.  4.1 


174 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


Table  170.    Neidhardt's  Fifth-Circle,  No.  10  (1/6-,  1/4-Comma) 

U--      --         h-l-i  -2  a      2  1  i,      5  1        Un  2 

Names  C°    C*    6D   4  E      6  E    3    F°      F f   3G  *  G     «  A~~2  B         B~3      C° 
Cents      0    94     198    298     392    498    596     696    796    894    996    1094     1200 

M.D.  3.0;  S.D.  3.8 


Table  171.    Neidhardt's  Fifth-Circle,  No.  10,  Idealized 

ji_J     _i        h+i        --Z-  u-J.      _i        u--       --5.      hn  _-l 

Names  C°    C#    6D   4  E      6  E    12  F°     F*    12G   4  G*   6  A    12B         B    12     C° 
Cents      0    94     198    298     398    498    598     696    796    896    996    1096    1200 

M.D.  1.3;  S.D.  2.4 

Table  172.    Neidhardt's  Sample  Temperament,  No.  2 
(1/12-,  1/6-,  1/4-Comma) 

Names    C°  Cw     D    l2  E        E    12  F    12  F      12G   6  G         A    3  B      12  B    12     C° 
Cents      0    90     194    294    386    496    590     698    792     890    994     1088    1200 

M.D.  6.3;  S.D.  7.2 

Temperament  10  (Table  170)  is  considerably  simpler,  with  two 
fifths  flat  by  1/4  comma,  three  by  1/6  comma,  and  the  remaining 
seven  pure.    The  deviation  is  slightly  lower  than  for  No.  7. 

But  in  No.  10  also  the  arrangement  is  far  from  symmetric. 
Let  us  see  what  would  result  from  an  approach  to  symmetry.  Al- 
though the  deviation  is  about  halved  in  Table  171,  it  is  possible 
that,  as  in  the  alphabetically  named  temperaments  by  Marpurg, 
the  least  deviation  for  all  four  of  these  Neidhardt  temperaments 
will  not  occur  with  the  most  nearly  symmetric  arrangement  of 
the  fifths. 

In  the  remaining  five  temperaments  in  this  group,  Neidhardt 
has  tempered  his  fifths  by  1/4,  1/5,  and  1/12  comma.  His  second 
and  third  "sample"  temperaments  (the  first  was  just  intonation) 
have  relatively  high  deviations. 42  No.  2  (Table  172)  has  three 
fifths  flat  by  1/4  comma,  one  by  1/5,  two  by  1/12,  five  pure,  and 
one  1/12  comma  sharp. 


42 

J.  Go  Neidhardt,  Gantzlich  erschopfte  mathematische  Abtheilung,  p.  34. 


175 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Neidhardt's  No.  3  (Table  173)  is  somewhat  less  erratic  than 
No.  2,  with  six  pure  fifths,  and  two  each  flat  by  1/4,  1/6,  or  1/12 
comma.   It  also  has  a  lower  deviation  than  No.  2. 

Rather  similar  to  the  above  sample  temperaments  is  his 
Third-Circle,  No.  1  (Table  174),  in  which  five  fifths  are  pure, 
two  flat  by  1/4  comma,  one  by  1/6,  and  four  by  1/2. 

Two  temperaments  from  the  Fifth-Circle  are  considerably 
better  than  the  three  just  mentioned.  In  No.  11  (Table  175)  there 
are  no  pure  fifths;  two  fifths  are  flat  by  1/4  comma,  two  by  1/6, 
five  by  1/12,  while  three  are  1/12  comma  sharp. 

Table  173.    Neidhardt's  Sample  Temperament,  No.  3 
(1/12-,  1/6-,  1/4-Comma) 

»     U       _i        ho— i.  5  U     2  x       H     H         -1      ho  5 

Names    C°Cff"12D    3  E      12E-'5    F°     Fff "6  G"5  Gff "  12A"  12B  B   6      C° 

Cents      0    92     196    296     388    498    592     698    794    892    996    1090    1200 

M.D.  5.7;  S.D.  6.4 

Table  174.    Neidhardt's  Third-Circle,  No.  1  (1/12-,  1/6-,  1/4-Comma) 

Names    C°    C      6D   4  E      12E    4    F°      F      8G    12  G      12A    2  B      12  B   4      C° 
Cents      0    94     198    296     390    498    592     700    794    894    998    1092    1200 

M.D.  5.3;  S.D.  5.9 

Table  175.    Neidhardt's  Fifth-Circle,  No.  11  (1/12- ,  1/6-,  1/4-Comma) 

u-2      -J      h+—     -—       +—       4--1       -X      u-3      _J        ho  -- 

Names    C°    C*    4D    4  E      12E    12  F    12  Fff    12G    12Gff    3A    2    B  B    2      C° 

Cents      0    96     198    296     394     500    598     700    800    894    996    1098     1200 

M.D.  2.7;  S.D.  3.2 

Table  176.    Neidhardt's  Fifth-Circle,  No.  12  (1/12- ,  1/6- ,  1/4-Comma) 

Names    C°    C^  12D~4  E      4  E_2    F°      Fff"2  G"  12G     4  A~4  B  B"2      C° 

Cents      0    100    198    300    396    498    600    700    798    900    996    1098    1200 

M.D.  2.0;  S.D.  2.3 


176 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


In  No.  12  (Table  176)  there  are  six  pure  fifths,  and  two  each 
flat  by  1/4,  1/6,  or  1/12  comma.  This  has  precisely  the  same 
number  of  each  size  of  fifth  as  the  third  sample  temperament, 
in  which  the  deviation  was  almost  three  times  as  great.  The 
reason,  of  course,  is  to  be  found  in  the  symmetry  of  No.  12. 


Metius'  System 

At  the  beginning  of  this  chapter  it  was  said  that  "by  making 
the  bounds  sufficiently  elastic"  all  irregular  systems  could  be 
classified.  That  statement  is  severely  tested  by  the  final  tuning 
method  listed  in  this  part  of  the  chapter,  one  presented  by  Ad- 
rian Metius.  It  was  not  possible  to  see  Metius'  own  description, 
and  Nierop,  who  gave  the  monochord,  seemed  to  have  been  puz- 
zled by  it  himself.  *  Nierop  has  shown  this  monochord  in  two 
forms,  one  from  1000  to  500  and  the  other  from  11520  to  5760, 
with  E  the  fundamental.  It  is  evident  from  the  context  that  the 
second  monochord  was  given  simply  to  show  how  its  lengths  have 
been  increased  or  diminished  by  arithmetic  divisions  of  the  syn- 
tonic comma,  and  that  only  the  first  table  comes  from  Metius 
directly., 

By  using  Metius'  lengths,  it  is  possible  to  reconstruct  the 
tempering,  indicated  by  the  exponents.  Apparently  there  is  only 
one  pure  fifth,  C-G.  The  fifths  on  BD  and  A  are  1/12  comma  flat, 
those  on  F  and  E  1/6  comma  flat,  on  B  and  C*  1/2  comma  flat, 
and  on  G  3/4  comma  flat!  The  fifths  on  D  and  F*  are  1/6  comma 
sharp,  that  on  D*  1/3  comma  sharp,  and  on  G*  1/2  comma  sharp. 

Metius'  system  does  not  seem  to  follow  any  known  system  of 
temperament  or  modification  thereof.  Specifically,  it  does  not 
resemble  the  meantone  temperament,  for  only  the  thirds  on  B*3 
and  E  are  pure,  the  other  thirds  varying  in  size  up  to  417  cents 
for  GD-BD  and  419  cents  for  AD-C.  But  there  is  no  pattern  ap- 
parent in  the  alterations,  no  planned  shift  from  good  to  poor  keys. 
The  fifth  G-D,  3/4  comma  flat,  is  almost  as  unsatisfactory  as 
this  same  fifth  would  be  in  just  intonation.  There  is  no  good  rea- 
son for  both  of  the  fifths  B-F#  and  C#-G#  to  be  half  a  comma  flat 

4**D,,  r.  van  Nierop,  Wis-konstige  Musyka  (Amsterdam,  1650).    The  reference 
here  is  to  page  60  of  the  2nd  edition  (1659). 

177 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


and  then  to  have  the  fifth  G#-D#  half  a  comma  sharp.    All  in  all, 
Metius  has  been  just  about  as  erratic  as  he  could  be. 

And  yet  the  system,  despite  its  irregularities,  is  much  bet- 
ter than  the  ordinary  1/4-comma  meantone  temperament  and  is 
slightly  better  than  the  Pythagorean  or  the  1/6 -comma  mean- 
tone.  That  much  we  must  grudgingly  admit.  Metius'  tempera- 
ment contains  eight  different  sizes  of  fifth.  But  that  is  not  much 
less  regular  than  many  of  the  fairly  good  temperaments  we  have 
shown  that  had  four  sizes  of  fifth,  while  Werckmeister's  Septe- 
nariumand  Neidhardt's  second  sample  temperament  had  five  dif- 
ferent sizes.  And  so  let  us  label  it  highly  irregular,  but  not 
really  unworkable. 

"Good"  Temperaments 

With  Metius'  enigmatic  temperament  we  have  described  the 
last  of  our  irregular  tuning  systems,  and  are  in  a  position  to  try 
to  formulate  a  judgment  upon  them.  It  is  easy  to  see  how  the 
modifications  of  the  Pythagorean,  just,  or  meantone  system  by 
the  halving  of  tones,  as  in  the  systems  of  Grammateus,  Ganassi, 
or  Artusi,  would  make  these  systems  much  more  like  equal  tem- 
perament. But  it  is  more  difficult  to  see  what  Werckmeister, 
Neidhardt,  and  Marpurg  were  driving  at  in  their  multifarious  at- 
tempts to  distribute  the  comma  unequally  among  the  twelve  fifths. 

If,  as  was  pointed  out  at  the  beginning  of  an  earlier  section 
of  this  chapter,  our  ideal  is  equal  temperament,  we  shall  praise 
highly  some  of  the  beautifully  symmetric  systems  of  Marpurg 
and  Neidhardt.  But  the  trouble  is  that  they  are  too  good!  The 
deviations  for  most  of  them  are  lower  than  for  a  piano  allegedly 
tuned  in  equal  temperament  by  the  most  skillful  tuner.  In  some 
cases  these  temperaments  might  have  been  successfully  trans- 
ferred from  paper  to  practice  by  calculating  the  number  of  beats 
for  each  of  the  beating  fifths.  Since  most  of  the  fifths  were  to  be 
tuned  pure,  such  a  method  might  have  been  easier  than  that  pur- 
sued today.  These  same  temperaments  might  have  been  reduced 
to  distances  on  a  monochord  with  slightly  greater  ease  than  equal 
temperament  could  be,  although  it  must  be  remembered  that  us- 
ually even  the  most  innocent  set  of  cents  values  needs  logarith- 

178 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


mic  computation  before  yielding  figures  for  a  monochord.  But  it 
will  be  safe  to  dismiss  most  of  these  oversubtle  systems  as  use- 
less, even  for  the  age  when  they  were  devised. 

What  do  we  have  left?  It  will  be  of  interest  to  consider  which 
of  his  twenty  systems  Neidhardt  considered  the  best.  In  the  Sec- 
tio  canonis  he  had  said,  "In  my  opinion,  the  first  [of  the  circu- 
lating temperaments]  is,  for  the  most  part,  suitable  for  a  vil- 
lage, the  second  for  a  town,  the  third  for  a  city,  and  the  fourth 
for  the  court."  The  fourth  was  equal  temperament;  the  mean 
deviations  of  the  other  temperaments  had  been  4.0,  3.3,  and  2.7 
cents,  respectively. 

In  the  much  later  Mathematische  Abtheilungen  Neidhardt  pre- 
sented eighteen  different  irregular  temperaments,  together  with 
just  intonation  and  equal  temperament.  He  then  attempted  to 
choose  the  best  of  these  twenty  tunings.  He  chose  equal  temper- 
ament, of  course,  and  the  two  temperaments  (Third-Circle,  No. 
2,  and  Fifth-Circle,  No.  8)  that  were  identical  with  the  first  and 
second  circulating  temperaments  above.  Now  half  of  the  rejected 
temperaments  had  deviations  lower  than  that  of  the  second  cir- 
culating temperament  (3.3),  and  a  couple  of  others  were  just 
about  as  good.  But  none  of  these  was  considered  worthy  in  the 
final  appraisal,  Neidhardt  had,  incidentally,  changed  his  ideas 
somewhat  as  to  the  relative  position  of  the  best  temperaments: 
the  Circulating  Temperament,  No.  2  (Fifth-Circle,  No.  8)  is  now 
considered  best  for  a  large  city;  No.  1  (Third-Circle,  No,  2)  for 
a  small  city;  and  Third-Circle,  No.  1,  not  included  before,  for  a 
village. 

If  we  examine  the  deviations  of  the  major  thirds  in  the  three 
temperaments  Neidhardt  himself  considered  superior,  we  quickly 
find  why  he  liked  them.  In  the  second  circulating  temperament 
(Table  155)  the  thirds  on  Cand  Fare  8  cents  sharper  than  a  pure 
third,  and  the  sharpness  gradually  increases  in  both  directions 
around  the  circle  of  fifths  until  the  three  worst  thirds  are  18 
cents  sharp.  In  the  first  circulating  temperament  (Table  151) 
the  third  on  C  is  only  6  cents  sharp,  and  there  is  the  same  grad- 
ual increase  until  the  five  poorest  thirds  are  all  18  cents  sharp. 
In  the  Third-Circle,  No.  1  (Table  174),  the  third  on  C  is  4  cents 
sharp,  and  the  six  poorest  thirds  are  either  18  or  20  cents  sharp. 

179 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT. 


Werckmeister' s  third  temperament,  the  first  of  the  three  he 
has  labeled  "correct"  (Table  140),  is  much  like  the  Neidhardt 
temperament  just  mentioned.  Its  thirds  on  C  and  F  are  only  4 
cents  sharp,  but  the  thirds  of  the  principal  triads  in  the  key  of 
D*3  are  all  a  syntonic  comma,  22  cents,  sharp.  Werckmeister 
himself  said  that  some  people  who  advocated  equal  temperament 
held  that  "in  the  future ...  it  will  be  just  the  same  to  play  an  air 
in  D*3  as  in  C.  ^  But  he  held  consistently  "that  one  should  let 
the  diatonic  thirds  be  somewhat  purer  than  the  others  that  are 
seldom  used." 45 

A  good  comparison  can  be  made  between  two  temperaments 
of  Neidhardt,  already  mentioned  as  having  fifths  of  four  different 
sizes  and  the  same  number  of  each  size,  but  with  a  different  ar- 
rangement. The  Fifth-Circle,  No.  12  (Table  176)  has  a  sym- 
metric arrangement  and  a  low  mean  deviation,  2.0.  Its  thirds 
show  no  trend  whatever  from  near  to  far  keys,  but  are  sufficiently 
irregular  to  make  this  seem  a  poor  attempt  at  equal  tempera- 
ment. Not  so  its  companion,  the  third  sample  temperament  (Ta- 
ble 173),  in  which  the  third  on  C  is  only  2  cents  sharp,  whereas 
four  of  the  five  poorest  thirds  are  20  cents  sharp.  To  be  sure, 
the  deviation  for  this  temperament,  5.7,  is  almost  three  times 
as  great  as  for  the  other  one,  and  there  is  a  painful  lack  of  sym- 
metry. But  the  unsymmetric  temperament  is  "circulating,"  and 
therefore  deserves  an  honored  place  among  the  "good"  temper- 
aments,, 

Thomas  Young's  temperaments  also  deserve  mention  for  their 
circulating  nature.  His  first  temperament  (Table  152)  is  equiva- 
lent to  a  temperament  with  four  pure  fifths  and  four  fifths  each 
tempered  by  1/6  or  1/12  comma.  It  is  constructed  with  scien- 
tific accuracy  so  that  the  thirds  range  in  sharpness  from  6  cents 
forC-E  to  22  cents,  a  syntonic  comma,  forF#-A#.  Its  mean  de- 
viation is  5.3.  On  the  other  hand,  there  is  the  symmetric  form 
of  this  temperament,  Marpurg's  D  (Table  154),  with  a  mean  de- 
viation of  1.3.   And  the  even  better,  nonsymmetric  form,  with  a 

"A.  Werckmeister,  Hypomnemata  musica  (Quedlinburg,  1697),  p.  36. 

45Werckmeister,     Musicalische    Paradoxal-Discourse     (Quedlinburg,    1707), 
p.  113. 

180 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


mean  deviation  of  0.3!  But  these  last-mentioned  temperaments 
are  curiosities  only,  whereas  Young's  differentiated  admirably 
between  near  and  far  keys. 

However,  Young's  first  temperament  was  too  difficult  to  con- 
struct, as  he  had  described  it  with  fifths  tempered  by  3/16  and 
"approximately"  1/12  syntonic  comma.  Therefore  he  substituted 
his  second  method  (Table  145),  which  was  of  the  utmost  simplic- 
ity, with  six  consecutive  perfect  fifths  and  six  consecutive  fifths 
tempered  by  1/6  ditonic  comma.  Its  mean  deviation  was  6.0.  In 
it  the  thirds  on  C,  G,  and  Dare  each  6  cents  sharp,  whereas  those 
on  F%  C#,  and  G*  are  each  22  cents  sharp.  Neidhardt's  Fifth- 
Circle,  No.  3  (Marpurg's  F)  is  the  symmetric  version  of  this 
temperament  (Table  147),  with  a  mean  deviation  of  2.0.  Again 
we  may  well  say  that  Young's  version  is  an  excellent  irregular 
temperament,  while  the  symmetrical  version  represents  having 
fun  with  figures. 

So  many  versions  of  good  circulating  temperaments  have  ap- 
peared on  these  pages,  each  with  its  points  of  excellence,  that  we 
cannot  resist  the  temptation  to  close  this  chapter  with  an  irreg- 
ular temperament  to  end  irregular  temperaments!  Gallimard's 
modification  of  the  ordinary  meantone  temperament,  by  a  sys- 
tematic variation  in  the  size  of  the  chromatic  fifths,  was  good 
enough  in  principle,  but  could  not  have  been  too  successful  be- 
cause of  the  large  number  of  other  fifths  tempered  by  1/4  comma. 

What  is  really  needed,  in  order  to  have  a  more  orderly  change 
in  the  size  of  the  thirds,  is  to  have  the  variable  tempering  ap- 
plied to  all  the  fifths,  instead  of  to  only  five  of  them.  Let  the 
fifth  D- A  be  the  flattest,  and  let  each  succeeding  fifth  in  both  di- 
rections around  the  circle  of  fifths  be  a  little  sharper  until  the 
fifth  on  A^  is  the  sharpest.  Then  the  total  parts  to  be  added  will 
be  1  +  2  +  3  +  4  +  5  +  6  +  5  +  4  +  3  +  2  +  1  =  36  parts.  Since  these 
parts  are  to  be  added  to  12  fifths,  it  is  evident  that  D-A,  the  flat- 
test fifth,  will  be  flatter  than  the  fifth  of  equal  temperament  by 
three  of  these  parts;  the  fifths  B-F#  and  F-C  will  be  precisely 
the  size  of  the  equal  fifth;  and  the  sharpest  fifth,  AD-E°,  will  be 
larger  than  the  equal  fifth  by  three  parts.  The  thirds  will  vary 
as  follows  (the  error  being  expressed  as  the  number  of  parts  be- 
low or  above  the  third  of  equal  temperament):  C-E,  -8;  G-B,  -8; 


181 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


D-F#,   -6;    A-C#,   -2:    E-G#,  2;    B-D#,  6;    Gb-Bb,  8;    Db-F,  8; 
Ab-C,  6;  Eb-G,  2;  Bb-D,  -2;   F-A,  -6. 

We  can  choose  the  value  for  one  part  that  will  give  the  de- 
sired size  of  thirds.  If  the  part  is  one  cent,  the  fifth  D-A  is  697 
cents,  practically  a  meantone  fifth,  and  the  fifth  Ab-Eb  is  703, 
practically  perfect;  the  best  thirds,  C-E  and  G-B,  are  392,  1/4 
comma  sharp;  the  poorest  thirds,  Gb-Bb  and  Db-F,  are  408, 
precisely  a  Pythagorean  third. 

Table  178  should  have  satisfied  the  desire  of  Werckmeister 
and  his  contemporaries  for  a  circulating  temperament  in  which 
all  the  thirds  are  sharp,  but  none  more  than  a  comma,  and  all 
the  fifths  are  flat  or  pure.  As  the  size  of  the  part  is  reduced, 
the  tuning  approaches  equal  temperament.  When  the  part  is  in- 
creased to  1  3/4  cents,  the  best  thirds  are  pure.  But  the  poor- 
est thirds  are  now  414  cents,  about  5/4  comma  sharp.  Thus  Ta- 
ble 178  probably  represents  the  limit  of  a  tolerable  temperament 
in  the  extreme  keys.  Since  the  mean  deviation  for  the  entire  se- 
ries of  temperaments  formed  in  this  manner  is  precisely  pro- 
portional to  the  size  of  the  part,  it  would  be  easy  to  devise  a  sys- 
tem with  the  deviation  of  any  of  the  systems  in  this  chapter,  but 
with  a  more  orderly  distribution  of  the  errors,  as  regards  com- 
mon keys  and  less-used  keys. 

The  Temperament  by  Regularly  Varied  Fifths  may  be  re- 
garded as  the  ideal  form  of  Werckmeister' s  "correct"  temper- 
aments and  of  Neidhardt's  "circulating"  temperaments  and  of  all 
"good"  temperaments  that  practical  tuners  have  devised  by  rule 
of  thumb.  Let  us  see,  therefore,  how  closely  it  is  approached  by 
these  other  temperaments.  In  Table  179,  the  deviations  have 
been  computed,  not  only  from  equal  temperament,  but  also  from 
our  temperament  with  variable  fifths.  The  table  shows  clearly 
that  the  temperaments  with  greatest  symmetry  do  not  fit  so  well 
into  the  desired  pattern  as  do  those  that  are  much  less  regular 
in  their  construction.  In  general,  the  temperaments  with  lowest 
deviation  from  the  one  ideal  temperament  will  have  a  high  devi- 
ation from  the  other.  Neidhardt's  second  circulating  tempera- 
ment has  the  unique  position  of  ranking  the  same  with  regard  to 
both. 


182 


IRREGULAR  SYSTEMS 


Table  177.    Metius'  Irregular  Temperament 


Lengths    1000    940   896    837    800    749    704    668    628    596    563    530    500 

"3  G#_1  A+~&  Bb+T5B 
M.D.  9.5;  S.D.  11.6 


Names        E°       F    6  F#    3  G    3  Gff      A    12  B      12B    6  C    3  C#    2D  12D#~2  E° 


Table  178.    Temperament  by  Regularly  Varied  Fifths 

Names     CxDxEFxGxAxB         C 
Cents       0     92     197    297    392    500    591    699    794    894    999    1091    1200 

M.D.  5.8;  S.D.  6.6 

Table  179.    Deviations  of  Certain  Temperaments 


From 

Equal 

Temperament 

From  Varied  Fifths 

M.D. 

S.D. 

M.D. 

S.D. 

Neidhardt's  Circulating,  No.  1 

4.0 

4.6 

2.1 

2.3 

No.  2 

3.3 

3.7 

3.3 

3.7 

No.  3 

2.7 

2.9 

4.2 

4.7 

Third- Circle,  No.  1 

5.3 

5.9 

1.2 

1.5 

Wer  ckmeister  's 

Correct,  No.  1 

6.0 

7.5 

1.9 

2.3 

No.  2 

9.2 

10.7 

4.7 

5.7 

No.  3 

5.0 

5.7 

3.8 

4.2 

Neidhardt's  Fifth-Circle,  No.  12 

2.0 

2.3 

6.2 

6.7 

Sample,  No.  3 

5.7 

6.4 

1.5 

1.8 

Young's                               No.  1 

5.3 

5.9 

1.7 

1.9 

Marpurg's                     Letter  D 

1.3 

1.6 

6.7 

7.1 

Young's                               No.  2 

6.0 

6.8 

1.9 

2.0 

Neidhardt's  Fifth-Circle,  No.  3 

2.0 

2.0 

5.0 

5.8 

Schlick's  (Hypothetical) 

8.0 

8.6 

2.7 

3.1 

Neidhardt's 

Third- Circle,  No.  3 

3.3 

4.7 

3.0 

3.8 

Our  hypothetical  reconstruction  of  Arnold  Schlick's  tempera- 
ment had  the  same  size  of  fifths  as  Neidhardt's  Third-Circle,  No. 
3,  but  differently  arranged,  and  with  a  fairly  high  deviation.  Ob- 
serve that,  with  this  other  standard  of  varied  dissonance,  Schlick's 
temperament  is  even  a  little  better  than  Neidhardt's.  Of  all  the 
temperaments  shown  in  our  table,  Neidhardt's  Third-Circle,  No. 

183 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


1  seems  to  be  the  best,  with  our  new  standard,  although  Neid- 
hardt  himself  said  it  was  best  for  a  village!  But  it  would  have 
been  difficult  to  tune,  and  therefore  Thomas  Young's  Tempera- 
ment, No.  2  probably  cannot  be  surpassed  from  the  practical 
point  of  view.  Even  so,  the  highest  honor  must  be  paid  to  old 
Arnold  Schlick,  writing  so  long  before  these  other  men,  but  stat- 
ing as  clearly  as  need  be  for  his  very  practical  purpose,  "Al- 
though they  will  all  be  too  high,  it  is  necessary  to  make  the  three 
thirds  C-E,  F-A,  and  G-B  better,  „ .  .as  much  as  the  said  thirds 
are  better,  so  much  will  G    be  worse  to  E  and  B." 


Table  180.    Compass  of  the  Lute 


G  Tuning 

A  Tuning 

0 

1 

2 

3   4  5   6 

7 

8 

0 

1 

2   3   4      5 

6 

7  8 

1. 

G 

Ab 

A 

Bb  B  C  Db(C#) 

D 

Eb 

A 

Bb 

B  C  C#(Db)  D 

Eb 

E  F 

2. 

D 

Eb 

E 

F   F#  G  Ab(G#) 

A 

Bb 

E 

F 

F#  G  G#(Ab)  A 

Bb 

B  C 

3. 

A 

Bb 

B 

C   C#D  Eb(D#) 

E 

F 

B 

C 

C#  D  D#(Eb)  E 

F 

F#  G 

4. 

F 

Gb 

G 

Ab  A  Bb  Cb(B) 

C 

Db 

G 

Ab 

A  BbB  (Cb)  C 

Db 

D  Eb 

5. 

C 

Db 

D 

Eb  E  F  Gb(F#) 

G 

Ab 

D 

Eb 

E  F  F#(Gb)  G 

Ab 

A  Bb 

6. 

G 

Ab 

A 

Bb  B  C  Db(C#) 

D 

Eb 

A 

Bb 

B  C  C#(Db)  D 

Eb 

E  F 

184 


Chapter  VIII.    FROM  THEORY  TO  PRACTICE 


In  our  intensive  study  of  scores  of  tuning  systems  we  have  failed 
to  note  what  may  be  learned  from  the  music  itself.  Some  of  the 
theorists  who  have  written  on  tuning  were  able  composers  as  well. 
When  they  described  with  precision  a  particular  division  of  the 
monochord,  their  theory  may  well  have  coincided  with  fact.  But 
the  tuning  theories  of  the  mere  mathematicians  do  not  carry  so 
much  weight.  Nor  do  the  rules  of  thumb  the  musicians  more 
commonly  presented.  All  of  these  theories  may  be  put  into  neat 
little  pigeonholes,  but  one  can  be  sure  that  the  practice  itself, 
because  of  the  limitations  of  the  human  ear,  was  even  more  var- 
ied than  the  extremely  varied  theories. 

It  is  not  to  be  expected  that  a  study  of  the  music  will  provide 
a  precise  picture  of  tuning  practice.  It  is  to  be  used  more  by 
way  of  corroborating  what  the  theorists  have  said.  Let  us  con- 
sider first  the  contention  of  Vicentino  that  the  fretted  instruments 
were  always  in  equal  temperament.  In  general  we  can  reach 
certain  conclusions  concerning  tuning  by  examining  the  range  of 
modulation.  However,  this  is  not  definitive  as  regards  the  lutes 
and  viols.  Korte  listed  D#'s  in  lute  music  from  1508,  an  A*  from 
1523,  and  many  D^'s  from  1529.*  But  the  mere  presence  of 
notes  beyond  the  usual  12 -note  compass  proves  little,  because 
the  lutes  were  not  restricted  to  a  total  compass  of  12  semitones. 
As  shown  in  Table  180,  the  normal  compass  with  the  G  tuning 
was  Ct>  to  C#  and  for  the  A  tuning  from  DD  to  D#. 

Ordinarily,  lutes  and  viols  had  six  strings,  tuned  by  fourths, 
with  a  major  third  in  the  middle.  Thus  the  open  strings  might 
be  G  C  F  A  D  G  or  A  D  G  B  E  A.  It  is  easy  to  see  here  the 
prototype  of  Sch'dnberg's  chords  built  by  fourths.  Because  of  the 
perfect  fourths,  the  fretted  instruments  might  have  inclined  to- 
ward the  Pythagorean  tuning,  as  the  later  violins  have  done. 
Mersenne  pointed  out  that  the  major  third  in  the  middle  would 


iOskar  Korte,  "Laute  und  Lautenmusik  bis  zur  Mitte  des  16.    Jahrhunderts," 
Internationale  Musikgesellschaft,  Beiheft  3  (1901). 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


then  be  sharp  by  a  comma. 

But  the  strings  of  lutes  and  viols  were  tuned  by  forming  uni- 
sons, fifths,  or  octaves  with  the  proper  frets  on  other  strings, 
thus  making  the  tuning  uniform  throughout  the  instrument.  Vin- 
cenzo  Galilei^  stated  that  if  the  tuning  were  not  equal,  semitones 
on  the  A  string  (mezzana)  of  the  lute  based  on  G  would  have  the 
note  names  shown  in  Table  180.  Since  the  frets  were  merely  pieces 
of  gut  tied  straight  across  the  fingerboards  at  the  correct  places, 
the  order  of  diatonic  and  chromatic  semitones  would  have  to  be 
the  same  on  all  strings.  Thus  the  chromatic  compass  of  a  lute 
with  six  strings  and  eight  frets  would  be  as  shown  in  Table  180, 
if  meantone  temperament  had  been  used. 

There  might  be  some  question  for  the  G  tuning  regarding 
notes  produced  by  the  6th  fret,  since  B  would  be  a  better  choice 
than  C"  on  the  4th  string.  But  the  remaining  notes  for  the  6th 
fret  agree  somewhat  better  with  other  notes  in  the  compass  than 
the  equivalent  sharped  notes  would  have  done.  Galilei  pointed 
out  that  Gt>  (4th  string,  1st  fret)  was  not  a  pure  fifth  to  C#  (3rd 
string,  4th  fret),  nor  was  DD  (5th  string,  1st  fret)  a  pure  octave 
to  the  C#.  He  might  have  added  that  DD  (1st  string,  6th  fret)  was 
not  a  pure  octave  above  the  C#  either. 

It  is  easy  to  multiply  examples  of  unsatisfactory  intervals  on 
the  unequally  tuned  lute  in  G.  (Read  them  a  tone  higher  for  the 
A  tuning.)  Try  building  major  triads  upon  the  notes  of  the  6th 
string,  starting  with  B13.  C,  D*3,  and  E*3  are  satisfactory  as  roots 
also,  but  false  triads  are  generated  on  B  and  D.  On  the  5th 
string,  starting  with  D,  the  satisfactory  triads  are  on  E13,  F,  and 
A13;  false  triads  on  D,  E,  Gb,  and  G.  On  the  4th  string,  starting 
with  G,  the  only  unsatisfactory  triad  is  on  C13.  On  the  3rd  string, 
starting  with  C,  the  other  satisfactory  triads  are  on  D,  E",  and 
F,  with  false  triads  on  C*  and  E.  Thus,  of  26  major  triads  in 
close  position,  only  17,  about  2/3,  are  available.  Some  of  the 
triads,  those  on  G,  D,  and  A,  unsatisfactory  in  the  lower  octave, 
can  be  played  correctly  in  the  higher  octave.  But  the  complete 
E  and  B  major  triads  are  unavailable  anywhere,  because  there 
are  no  G*  and  D#  —  unless,  of  course,  the  6th  fret  runs  to  sharps 
rather  than  to  flats. 


^Fronimo  (Venice  1581;  revised  edition,  1584),  pp.  103 f. 
186 


FROM  THEORY  TO  PRACTICE 


As  illustrations  of  incongruous  notes  on  particular  frets,  let 
us  examine  some  of  the  Austrian  lute  music  of  the  sixteenth  cen- 
tury, as  found  in  Volume  18  of  the  Austrian  Denkm'aler.  The 
first  collection  represented  is  Hans  Judenkiinig's  Ain  schone 
kunstliche  Underweisung  (1523),  His  third  Priamell  is  modal, 
but  often  suggests  C  minor.  Like  most  of  the  German  and  Aus- 
trian composers,  Judenkiinig  used  the  A  tuning  of  the  lute.  In 
bar  3  the  note  aD  appears  as  the  4th  fret  on  the  2nd  string,  indi- 
cating that  this  fret  has  a  flat  tuning  (see  Table  180).  But  in  bar 
4  there  is  a  b  and  in  bar  19  a  c#  ,  both  of  which  belong  to  the 
sharp  tuning  for  this  fret. 

For  Judenkiinig's  fourth  Priamell  the  editor  has  put  the  sig- 
nature of  three  sharps,  as  an  indication  of  the  prevailing  sharp- 
ness. This  even  extends  to  the  6th  fret,  which  would  then  include 
an  e*  .  Actually  there  is  an  e#  in  the  music,  and  no  f '.  There- 
fore it  would  have  been  possible  to  play  this  piece  with  an  une- 
qual temperament,  but  not  without  changing  the  6th  fret  from  its 
normal  flat  tuning, 

Simon  Gintzler's  fifth  Recercar  (1547)  used  the  Italian  G  tun- 
ing.  Here  the  6th  fret  has  a  flat  tuning,  as  shown  by  aD  and  a 
very  frequent  eb  .  But  in  bar  10  there  is  a  b  instead  of  the  c*5 
belonging  to  the  flat  tuning.  In  Gintzler's  setting  of  Senfl's  song 
"Vita  in  ligno  moritur,"  the  6th  fret  is  again  flat,  but  in  bar  15 
both  ab   and  b  occur. 

The  a*3  and  b  also  occur  several  times  in  Bakf ark's  Fantas- 
ias (1565).  More  interesting  is  his  setting  of  "Veni  in  hortum 
meum,  soror  mea"  (1573).  In  bar  50,  d*  '  occurs  as  the  third  of 
the  B  major  triad,  indicating  a  sharp  tuning  for  the  6th  fret.  This 
means  that  f'  is  not  available  on  this  fret;  but  f '  does  occur  in 
bar  56  and  elsewhere.  In  bar  62  the  complete  C  minor  triad  oc- 
curs: c'  eb  g'  c",  with  the  eb'  the  4th  fret  on  the  3rd  string. 
But  this  fret  must  have  had  a  sharp  tuning,  since  the  notes  d#' 
g    ,  and  c      occur  on  it  with  great  frequency. 

It  would  be  easy  to  multiply  examples,  from  the  music  of  Ital- 
ian, French,  and  Spanish  composers.  Those  that  have  been  given 
are  sufficient  to  show  that  in  the  golden  age  of  lute  music  the 
composers  were  indifferent  to  discords  that  would  have  arisen 


187 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


if  an  unequal  temperament  had  been  used.  The  example  from 
Judenklinig  occurs  so  early  in  the  century  (1523)  that  it  seems 
very  probable  that  lutes  and  viols  did  employ  equal  temperament 
from  an  early  time,  perhaps  from  the  beginning  of  the  sixteenth 
century. 

We  need  not  be  too  much  concerned  with  what  the  equal  tem- 
perament for  the  fretted  instruments  was  really  like.  It  might 
have  been  the  Grammateus-Bermudo  tuning  —  Pythagorean  with 
mean  semitones  for  the  chromatic  notes*  It  might  have  been  the 
Ganassi-Reinhard  mean  semitones  applied  to  just  intonation,  or 
Artusi's  more  subtle  system  of  mean  semitones  in  meantone 
temperament.  Or  the  frets  might  have  been  placed  according  to 
Galilei's  18:17  ratio,  or  (correctly)  according  to  Salinas'  ratio 
of  the  12th  root  of  2.  In  any  case,  it  would  have  been  a  good, 
workable  temperament. 


Tuning  of  Keyboard  Instruments 

In  the  early  sixteenth  century  Schlick  and  Grammateus  de- 
scribed systems  for  keyboard  instruments  that  came  close  to 
equal  temperament,  and  the  correct  application  of  Lanfranco's 
tuning  rules  must  have  resulted  in  equal  temperament  itself. 
But  these  systems  were  anomalous  for  a  day  when  few  acciden- 
tals were  written.  Examples  of  organ  music  from  the  late  fif- 
teenth and  the  entire  sixteenth  century  are  found  in  numerous 
collections,  such  as  Schering's  Alte  Meister  aus  der  Friihzeit 
desOrgelspiels;  Volume  1  of  Bonnet's  Historical  Organ  Recitals; 
Kinkeldey's  Orgel  und  Clavier  in  der  Musik  des  16.  Jahrhun- 
derts;  Volume  1  of  Margaret  Glyn's  Early  English  Organ  Music; 
Volume  3  of  Torchi's  L'arte  musicale  in  Italia;  Wasielewski's 
Geschichte  der  Instrumentalmusik  im  16.  Jahrhundert;  Volume 
6  of  the  Italian  Classics  series. 

With  the  exception  of  the  English  composers,  the  compass 
used  by  all  these  composers  was  less  than  12  notes  —  ED-F*  or 
Bb-C  .  Both  Tallis  and  Redford  had  D#  in  one  piece  and  Eb  in 
another,  thus  posing  a  problem  with  regard  to  the  tuning.  But 
except  for  them,  there  was  no  problem  about  performance:    all 


188 


FROM  THEORY  TO  PRACTICE 


of  this  organ  music  could  have  been  played  on  an  instrument  in 
meantone  temperament. 

Even  12  of  Schlick's  14  little  pieces  (Monatshefte  fur  Musik- 
geschichte,  1869)  lie  within  a  compass  of  Eb-C#.  One  of  the  re- 
maining pieces  has  an  A^;  the  other,  G#.  Since  Schlick  had  di- 
rected that  the  wolf  be  divided  equally  between  the  fifths  C*-G* 
and  Ab-ED,  these  notes  would  have  caused  him  no  difficulty. 
Perhaps  Tallis  and  Redford  were  dividing  the  error  similarly. 

Much  the  same  can  be  said  for  the  clavier  music  of  this  pe- 
riod. Merian's  Per  Tanz  in  den  deutschen  Tabulaturbuchern 
(Leipzig,  1927)  contains  about  200  tiny  keyboard  pieces,  and  Vol- 
ume 2  of  Bbhme's  Geschichte  des  Tanzes  about  20  more.  None 
exceeds  the  Eb-G#  compass.  The  famous  English  collection  of 
virginal  music,  Parthenia,  reveals  nothing  beyond  the  fact  that 
Byrd  preferred"!^  the  younger  composers  Bull  and  Gibbons, 
D*.  In  Margaret  Glyn's  edition  of  Gibbons'  Complete  Keyboard 
Works,  five  of  the  33  virginal  pieces  have  a  D#,  but  only  two  con- 
tain  Eb's,  one  of  these,  a  Pavan  in  G  minor,  having  also  an  AD. 
But  that  does  not  necessarily  mean  that  Gibbons  did  not  use  the 
meantone  temperament.  The  virginals  could  have  been  set  for 
an  A13  at  one  time  and  for  a  D*  at  another  —  a  point  that  will  be 
discussed  at  some  length  later.  More  significant  are  the  A^  and 
D#  that  occur  in  a  G  minor  Fancy  for  organ  by  Gibbons.  Unless 
Gibbons'  tuning  was  appreciably  better  than  the  meantone  tem- 
perament, this  Fancy  would  have  had  some  very  rough  places. 
This  same  Ab-D#  was  used  in  Tarquinio  Merula's  Sonata  Cro- 
matica,  a  work  having  a  modern  ring  because  of  its  chromati- 
cism.3 

Just  a  word  about  chromaticism.  Other  things  being  equal, 
a  piece  that  contains  many  chromatic  progressions  is  more  likely 
to  have  an  excessive  tonal  compass  than  one  that  is  not  chro- 
matic. But,  since  there  are  12  different  pitch  names  in  the  mean- 
tone  compass,  Eb-G#,  it  is  entirely  possible  for  a  chromatic 
piece  to  lie  within  it.  A  Toccata  by  Michelangelo  Rossi,  for  ex- 
ample, published  in  1657,  is  very  chromatic,  but  carefully  re- 


3Luigi  Torchi,  L/arte  musicale  in  Italia  (Milan,  post  1897),  III,  345-352. 

189 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


mains  within  the  meantone  bounds.4 

The  great  English  manuscript  source  of  the  early  seventeenth 
century,  the  Fitzwilliam  Virginal  Book,  is  a  monument  to  the 
boldness  of  the  clavier  composers  of  that  time.  Naylor^  has 
given  a  fascinating  and  exhaustive  account  of  the  music  in  this 
collection,  and  has  shown  that  many  of  the  progressions  contain- 
ing accidentals  resemble  modulations  to  our  major  and  minor 
keys  more  than  they  do  modal  cadences.  Twenty-five  of  the  297 
compositions  contain  D*'s,  with  Bull,  Byrd,  Farnaby,  and  Tom- 
kins  in  the  lead0  Bull,  Farnaby,  Tisdall,  and  Oystermayre  have 
A#'s  also. 

With  one  exception,  the  largest  compass  in  the  entire  collec- 
tion is  that  of  Byrd's  "Ut,  re,  mi,  fa,  sol,  la,"  which  extends 
from  AD  to  D*0  That  exception,  of  course,  is  John  Bull's  com- 
position on  the  hexachord,  with  the  same  title  as  Byrd's.  It  over- 
laps the  circle  of  fifths  by  six  notes,  with  the  compass  CD-A*. 
Bull  states  his  Canto  Fermo  first  on  G  and  rises  by  tones  through 
A,  B,  Db,  Eb,  and  F.  He  then  begins  afresh  with  Ab,  Bb,  C,  D, 
E,  F%  and  G.  An  enharmonic  modulation  occurs  at  the  begin- 
ning of  Section  4,  where  the  chord  of  F#  is  quitted  as  GD.  The 
editors  of  the  Fitzwilliam  Virginal  Book  were  so  impressed  with 
this  passage  that  they  correctly  stated  in  a  footnote, "This  inter- 
esting experiment  in  enharmonic  modulation  is  thus  tentatively 
expressed  in  the  MS.;  the  passage  proves  that  some  kind  of 
'equal  temperament'  must  have  been  employed  at  this  date." 6 

This  remarkable  composition  is  not  a  mere  juggling  with 
sounds,  as  Nay  lor  has  alleged.  It  has  real  musical  interest,  and 
because  of  its  sustained  style  seems  better  adapted  to  the  organ 
than  to  the  clavier.  But  do  not  try  to  build  up  a  theory  of  the  use 
of  equal  temperament  in  England  during  Queen  Elizabeth's  reign 
on  the  basis  of  Dr.  Bull's  composition.  Remember  that  it  stands 
practically  alone.  It  seems  almost  as  if  Bull  had  written  a  Fancy 
for  four  viols,  and  then,  led  by  some  mad  whim,  had  transcribed 

4 Ibid.,  p.  309. 

^An  Elizabethan  Virginal  Book  (London,  1905). 

"J.  A.  Fuller-Maitland  and  W.  Barclay  Squire,  The  Fitzwilliam  Virginal  Book 
(Leipzig,  1899),  I,  183. 

190 


FROM  THEORY  TO  PRACTICE 


it  for  virginals  and  tuned  his  instrument  to  suit. 

One  of  the  boldest  of  the  keyboard  composers  of  the  early 
seventeenth  century  was  Frescobaldi,  an  exact  contemporary  of 
Gibbons.  Of  his  31  works  for  organ  and  clavier,  •  three  contain 
DD,  three  a  D#,  and  one  an  A*.  One  of  the  most  interesting  of 
these  is  the  Partite  sopra  Passacagli  for  organ,  with  a  compass 
of  D^-G*.  The  G*  is  the  third  of  the  dominant  triad  of  A  minor, 
and  the  D*3  the  third  of  the  subdominant  triad  of  F  minor.  Hence 
the  ordinary  meantone  temperament  would  be  inadequate  for 
Frescobaldi. 

In  decided  contrast  to  Frescobaldi  are  Sweelinck  (German 
Denkm'aler,  IV  Band,  1.  Folge)  and  Scheidt  (German  Denkmaler, 
I  Band).  Sweelinck' s  Fantasia  Cromatica,  with  E°-D#  compass, 
was  the  only  one  of  36  pieces  examined  to  exceed  12  scale  de- 
grees, and  Scheidt,  although  not  averse  to  chromaticism  and 
rather  fond  of  D*'s,  had  no  single  composition,  of  44  examined, 
with  more  than  12  degrees. 

As  we  reach  the  middle  of  the  seventeenth  century,  we  shall 
have  to  differentiate  more  carefully  between  music  for  organ 
and  for  clavier.  The  organ  had  a  fixed  compass,  usually  Eb-G#, 
but  perhaps  B^-D*  or  AD-C^.  Even  if  the  composer  did  not  em- 
ploy AD  and  D#,  for  example,  in  the  same  composition,  as  Gib- 
bons and  Merula  had  done,  the  presence  of  these  notes  in  sepa- 
rate compositions  was  an  indication  that  he  was  using  at  least  a 
modified  version  of  the  meantone  temperament." 

Not  so  for  clavier.  A  study  of  the  accidentals  in  clavier 
music  suggests  that  tuning  practice  must  have  accommodated  it- 
self to  the  music  to  be  played.  The  performer  would  retune  when 
changing  from  sharp  to  flat  keys.  Bach  could  tune  his  entire 
harpsichord  in  fifteen  minutes;  to  change  the  pitches  of  only  a 
couple  of  notes  in  each  octave  would  have  taken  a  much  shorter 
time.    Moreover,  all  the  movements  of  the  common  dance  suites 


7I  classici  della  musica  italiana  (Milan,  1919),  Vol.  XII;  Torchi,  op.  cit., 
Vol.  III.  

Q 

°The  course  of  the  argument  and  most  of  the  examples  in  the  remaining  part 
of  this  section  have  been  taken  freely  from  my  article  "Bach  and  The  Art  of 
Temperament,"  Musical  Quarterly,  XXXIII  (1947),  64-89. 

191 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


were  in  the  same  key,  and  this  helped  to  restrict  the  compass  to 
not  more  than  twelve  different  pitch  names,  even  if  that  compass 
was  not  the  conventional  ED-G^. 

The  theorists  give  us  little  information  about  the  variable 
tuning  of  claviers.  Mersenne  hinted  at  the  practice^,  He  had 
given  two  keyboards  in  just  intonation,  the  first  with  sharps  only 
(except  for  BD)  and  the  second  with  flats.  Current  practice,  he 
said,  was  represented  by  either  of  these,  but  with  tempered,  not 
just,  intervals^  Some  eighty-five  years  later  Kuhnau  wrote  to 
Mattheson  that  the  strings  of  his  Pantalonisches  Cimbal  (a  large 
keyed  dulcimer)  vibrated  so  long  he  could  not  use  equal  temper- 
ament upon  it,  but  had  to  "correct  one  key  or  another"  when  turn- 
ing from  flats  to  sharps. 

More  valuable  evidence  of  the  variable  tuning  practice  for 
clavier  comes  from  the  music  itself.  Of  Froberger's  67  clavier 
compositions  (Austrian  Denkmaler,  VI,  2.  Theil,  and  X,  2.  Theil), 
6  use  14  scale  degrees,  10  use  13,  and  the  remaining  51  use  12 
or  fewer.  But  only  half  (26)  of  the  51  lie  wholly  within  the  usual 
meantone  compass.  His  accidentals  range  altogether  from  G^ 
to  E#. 

Similarly,  Johann  Pachelbel's  clavier  music  (Bavarian  Denk- 
maler, 2.  Jahrgang,  1.  Band)  suggests  a  variable  tuning.  Of  49 
compositions  examined,  only  2  have  more  than  12  scale  degrees. 
But  of  the  remaining  47,  only  21,  or  less  than  half,  lie  within  the 
E^-G*  compass,  and  the  total  range  is  from  D*3  to  B*.  An  ex- 
ception among  Pachelbel's  works,  the  Suite  in  AD  (Suite  ex  Gis), 
beginning  with  anAllemand  inAD  minor,  contains  an  enharmonic 
modulation  at  the  point  where  the  Fb  major  triad  is  treated  as  E 
major  by  resolving  upon  A  minor,  just  before  a  cadence  in  E*3 
major!  With  a  range  from  D*30  to  B  for  this  single  movement, 
it  seems  evident  that  for  the  moment  Pachelbel  was  as  reckless 
as  Bach. 

Kuhnau' s  works  (German  Denkmaler,  IV  Band,  1.  Folge)  give 
musical  evidence  of  variability  to  buttress  what  he  wrote  to  Mat- 
theson. Of  his  28  clavier  works,  3  of  the  6  Biblical  Sonatas  have 
a  compass  of  14  scale  degrees;  the  other  3  sonatas  and  5  other 
works  have  13.    But  of  the  remaining  17  works  that  have  no  more 


192 


FROM  THEORY  TO  PRACTICE 


than  12  different  pitches  in  the  octave,  only  2  lie  wholly  within 
the  Eb-G*  tuning.  Actually  Kuhnau  preferred  equal  temperament 
upon  the  clavier.  But  most  of  these  works  would  have  been  pass- 
able in  meantone  temperament  if  he  had  "corrected"  some  of  the 
notes,  just  as  he  did  on  the  Pantalon. 

Of  Frangois  Couperin's  27  charming  suites  for  clavecin,  only 
6  have  no  more  than  12  different  scale  degrees.  They  are  all  in 
the  minor  key,  and  in  each  the  flattest  note  is  a  semitone  higher 
than  the  keynote,  as  No.  8  in  B  minor  has  the  compass  C-E*. 
Twenty  of  the  remaining  21  suites  exceed  the  circle  of  fifths  by 
one  or  two  notes.  But  here  again  it  is  characteristic  to  have  the 
flattest  note  a  semitone  above  the  tonic.  For  example,  all  five 
suites  in  D  major-minor  have  the  precise  compass  Eb-A#.  Cou- 
perin  leaves  a  strong  impression  that  the  dissonance  inevitable 
in  the  slightly  extended  compass  was  a  coolly  calculated  risk, 
and  that  a  variable  meantone  tuning  was  used  for  these  suites 
also.  The  one  exception  is  No.  25,  in  Eb  major  and  C  major- 
minor.  The  compass  here  is  15  scale  degrees,  from  Gb  to  D*. 
This  would,  perhaps,  be  carrying  piquancy  too  far. 

There  is  ample  evidence  that  in  Italy  during  the  first  half  of 
the  eighteenth  century  equal  temperament  or  its  equivalent  was 
being  practiced.  Three  composers  represented  in  the  Italian 
Classics  had,  in  a  particular  composition,  a  similar  compass, 
15  notes  in  the  overlapping  circle  of  fifths.  They  are  Zipoli, 
Db-D#,  Vol.  36;  Serini,  Cb-C#,  Vol.  29;  and  Durante,  Gb-G#, 
Vol.  11.  Of  70  of  Domenico  Scarlatti's  delightful  little  "sona- 
tas,"9 45,  or  more  than  half,  overlap  the  circle.  In  one  sonata 
he  had  a  compass  of  18  degrees,  Db-B#;  in  another,  17,  Gb-A#. 
All  of  these  men  upon  occasion  wrote  notes  so  remote  from  the 
tonal  center  that  meantone  temperament  seems  wholly  out  of  the 
question.    Both  Serini  and  Durante  used  Fx,  and  Scarlatti,  Cx. 

At  this  time,  in  Germany,  Telemann  was  advocating  a  form 
of  multiple  division  with  55  notes  in  the  octave,  for  a  clavier 
with  only  12  notes  in  the  octave,  which  was  practically  the  same 
as  Silbermann's  1/6-comma  variety  of  meantone  temperament. 
We  might  expect,  therefore,  that  his  compositions  for  clavier 

9Heinrich  Barth,  Klavierwerke  von  Domenico  Scarlatti  (4  vols.;  Vienna,  c. 
1901).  ~ 

193 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


would  not  exceed  the  bounds  of  the  meantone  temperament.  How- 
ever, Telemann's  36  Clavier  Fantasies  have  a  total  range  of  G"- 
B#,  the  same  as  for  Couperin's  suites.  Only  8  of  the  fantasies 
overlap  the  circle,  by  one  or  two  degrees.  Of  the  remaining  28, 
only  one  lies  within  the  ordinary  meantone  bounds,  E"-G*.  The 
others  swing  to  the  sharp  side  or  the  flat  side,  depending  upon 
the  key.  Thus  Telemann  undoubtedly  used  the  meantone  temper- 
ament, but  with  variable  intonation. 

It  has  been  suggested  in  the  preceding  pages  that  composers 
such  as  Bull,  Gibbons,  Frescobaldi,  and  Domenico  Scarlatti, 
whose  works  exceed  the  meantone  bounds  by  several  scale  de- 
grees, were  not  using  the  meantone  temperament.  Were  they, 
then,  using  equal  temperament?  That  question  is  difficult  to  an- 
swer, especially  since  there  was  a  type  of  tuning  that  would  have 
been  fairly  satisfactory  in  many  of  these  cases.  The  title  of 
Bach's  great  collection  of  preludes  and  fugues,  Das  wohltem- 
perirte  Clavier,  has  usually  been  taken  to  mean,  as  Parry  called 
it,  "The  Clavichord  Tuned  in  Equal  Temperament."  But  even  in 
Bach's  day  there  was  a  good  German  phrase  for  equal  tempera- 
ment —  "die  gleichschwebende  Temperatur,"  "the  equally  beat- 
ing temperament."  Bach's  title  might  better  be  paraphrased, 
"The  Well-Tuned  Piano." 

Now,  "well -tuned"  had  been  used  in  a  somewhat  technical 
sense  by  the  Flemish  mathematician  Simon  Stevin,  over  a  cen- 
tury before  the  first  volume  of  the  "48"  was  compiled  in  1722, 
and  by  Bach's  great  French  contemporary  Rameau  also,  with  a 
meaning  nearly  the  same  as  Parry  has  given  to  it.  To  German 
theorists,  however,  there  was  a  distinction.  Andreas  Werck- 
meister  has  erroneously  been  hailed  as  the  father  of  equal  tem- 
perament because  of  the  title  of  one  of  his  works  on  tuning, 
Musicalische  Temperatur,  and  because  of  Mattheson's  eulogy. 
Mattheson  had  said,  "And  thus  the  fame  previously  divided  between 
Werckmeister  and  Neidhardt  remains  ineradicable  —  that  they 
brought  temperament  to  the  point  where  all  keys  could  be  played 
without  offense  to  the  ear."^    (Underscoring  is  the  present  au- 


10J.  Mattheson.  Critica  musica.  II,  162, 
194 


FROM  THEORY  TO  PRACTICE 


thor's.)  Werckmeister  himself  has  used  the  phrase  "wohl  tem- 
perirt"  as  follows:  "But if  we  have  a  well-tuned  clavier,  we  can 
play  both  the  major  and  minor  modes  on  every  note  and  transpose 
them  at  will.  To  one  who  is  familiar  with  the  entire  range  of  keys, 
this  affords  variety  upon  the  clavier  and  falls  upon  the  ear  very 
pleasantly." 

What  did  Werckmeister  mean  by  these  words?  To  use  Neid- 
hardt's  phrase,  he  meant  a  "completely  circulating  genus,"  that 
is,  a  tuning  in  which  one  could  circumnavigate  the  circle  of  fifths 
without  mal  de  son,  Both  men,  as  we  have  seen  in  Chapter  VII, 
presented  a  number  of  different  monochords,  with  the  "foreign" 
thirds  beating  as  much  as  a  comma.  Werckmeister  said  of  them, 
"It  would  be  very  easy  to  let  the  thirds  Db-F,  Gb-Bb,  Ab-C  beat 
less  than  a  full  comma;  but  since  thereby  the  other,  more  fre- 
quently used  thirds  obtain  too  much,  it  is  better  that  the  latter 
should  remain  purer,  and  the  harshness  be  placed  upon  those 
that  are  used  the  least."  Elsewhere  Werckmeister  described 
equal  temperament  with  fair  accuracy,  but  demurred,  "I  have 
hitherto  not  been  able  to  approve  this  idea,  because  I  would  rather 
have  the  diatonic  keys  purer."  And  so  to  Werckmeister  "well- 
tuned"  meant  "playable  in  all  keys  —but  better  in  the  keys  more 
frequently  used." 

If,  then,  a  composer  exceeded  twelve  different  pitch  names 
rarely  and  then  only  by  a  few  scale  degrees,  his  works  could  have 
been  played  to  good  advantage  on  a  "well-tuned  clavier."  Com- 
posers like  Bull  and  Pachelbel  and  Scarlatti,  however,  who  ef- 
fected enharmonic  modulations  and  used  double  sharps,  would 
have  been  badly  served  even  by  Werckmeister' s  best-known 
"correct"  temperament,  in  which  the  key  of  Db  had  Pythagorean 
thirds  for  all  its  major  triads.  Equal  temperament  was  needed 
for  their  works.  -^ 

An  equal  temperament  was  needed  for  the  keyboard  works  of] 
Bach,  both  for  clavier  and  for  organ.  It  is  generally  agreed  that 
Bach  tuned  the  clavier  equally.  Actually  he  was  opposed  to  equal 
temperament,  in  the  sense  that  there  must  be  strict  mathemati- 
cal ratios,  which  are  first  applied  to  the  monochord  and  from 
there  to  the  instrument  to  be  tuned.  Of  course  he  was  right.  The 
best  way  to  tune  in  equal  temperament,  as  Ellis  stated,  is  to 

195 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


count  beats.  Have  you  ever  heard  of  a  contemporary  piano  tuner 
who  carried  a  monochordwith  him?  And  yet  the  underlying  the- 
ory must  be  correct  or  the  result  will  be  unsatisfactory:  Ellis 
could  not  have  given  his  practical  tuning  rule  with  assurance  had 
he  not  been  able  to  calculate  accurately  how  far  its  use  would 
fall  short  of  the  perfection  implied  by  the  term  "equal  tempera- 
ment." 

The  organ  works  of  Bach  show  as  great  a  range  of  modulation 
as  his  clavier  works  do.  Except  for  a  dozen  chorale  preludes  in 
the  Orgelbiichlein,  there  are  only  3  organ  works  of  148  examined 
that  do  not  overstep  the  compass  of  the  conventionally  tuned  or- 
gan. The  compass  of  individual  organ  pieces  is  very  frequently 
13,  14,  and  15  scale  degrees,  and  even  18,  19,  and  21  degrees 
have  been  observed.  The  compass  of  Bach's  organ  works  as  a 
whole  is  E°k-Cx,  25  degrees!  In  these  works  is  a  host  of  ex- 
amples of  triads  in  remote  keys  that  would  have  been  dreadfully 
dissonant  in  any  sort  of  tuning  except  equal  temperament.  For 
corroboration,  if  corroboration  be  necessary,  we  need  but  note 
the  advice  that  Sorge  gave  to  the  instrument-maker  Silbermann, 
two  years  before  Bach's  death.  Sorge,  a  proponent  of  equal  tem- 
perament, said:  "In  a  word  —  Silbermann' s  way  of  tempering 
cannot  exist  with  modern  practice.  I  call  upon  all  impartial  and 
experienced  musicians  —  especially  the  world-famous  Herr  Bach 
in  Leipzig  —  to  witness  that  this  is  all  the  absolute  truth.    It  is 

to  be  desired,  therefore,  that  the  excellent  man  [Silbermann] 

should  alter  his  opinion  regarding  temperament. . .  .     * 

Just  Intonation  in  Choral  Music 

We  have  seen  that  just  intonation  exists  in  many  different 
forms,  and  that  the  best  version,  if  modulations  are  to  be  made 
to  keys  beyond  B^  and  A,  comes  near  the  Pythagorean  tuning,  as 
with  Ramis.  The  contention  has  often  been  made  that  unaccom- 
panied voices  sing  in  just  intonation.    Zarlino-^    listed  instru- 

11Georg  Andreas  Sorge,    Gesprach  zwischen    einem  Musico  theoretico   und 
einem  Studioso  musices,  p.  21. 

12Sopplimenti  musicali,  Chaps.  33-37. 
196 


FROM  THEORY  TO  PRACTICE 


merits  in  three  groups,  each  with  a  different  tuning:  keyboard 
instruments  in  meantone  temperament;  fretted  instruments  in 
equal  temperament;  voices,  violins,  and  trombones  in  just  in- 
tonation. His  argument  was  that  since  intonation  is  free  for  these 
three  last-named  groups,  they  would  use  an  intonation  in  which 
thirds  and  sixths  are  pure.  Three  hundred  and  forty-eight  years 
later  Lindsay  Norden  said,  "As  we  shall  show,  no  singer  can  sing 
a  cappella  in  any  temperament ....  A  cappella  music,  therefore, 
is  always  sung  in  just  or  untempered  intonation.     ^ 

Let  us  see  what  is  implied  by  these  statements.  In  the  first 
place,  singers  must  be  able  to  sing  the  thirds  and  sixths  purely.^ 
This  may  sound  like  a  self-evident  truth,  too  absurd  to  discuss. 
But  scientific  studies  of  intonation  preferences  show  that  the  hu- 
man ear  has  no  predilection  for  just  intervals,  not  even  the  pure 
major  third.  °  Alexander  Ellis  declared  that  it  was  unreliable 
to  tune  the  pure  major  thirds  of  meantone  temperament  directly, 
preferring  results  obtained  by  beating  fifths.  Hence  the  singers 
must  be  highly  trained  to  be  able  to  sing  the  primary  triads  of  a 
key  justly. 

In  the  second  place,  the  singers  must  be  able  to  differentiate 
intervals  differing  by  the  syntonic  comma,  1/9  tone.  We  have 
seen  that  inPtolemy's  version  of  the  syntonic  tuning  theDminor 
triad,  the  supertonic  triad  of  the  key  of  C  major,  will  be  false. 
If,  as  Kornerup  and  others  advocate,  the  Didymus  tuning  is  used 
instead  of  Ptolemy's,  the  dominant  triad  will  be  false,  which  is 
a  greater  loss„  But  a  singer  trained  to  niceties  of  intonation 
would  have  to  vary  his  pitch  by  a  comma  in  such  critical  places, 
and  thus  save  the  situation.    Very  good.    But  studies  at  the  Uni- 

13n.  Lindsay  Norden,  "A  New  Theory  of  Untempered  Music,"  Musical  Quar- 
terly, XXII  (1936),  218. 

14Except  for  the  reference  to  the  Italian  madrigalists,  the  remaining  part  of 
this  section  has  been  freely  adapted  from  my  article  "Just  Intonation  Con- 
futed," Music  and  Letters,  XIX  (1938),  48-60  by  permission  of  the  editor  of 
Music  and  Letters,  18  Great  Marlborough  Street,  London,  W.  1. 

15Paul  C.  Greene,  "Violin  Intonation,"  Journal  of  the  Acoustical  Society  of 
America,  IX  (1937-38),  43-44;  Arnold  M.  Small,  "Present-Day  Preferences 
for  Certain  Melodic  Intervals  . . . ,"  Ibid.,  X  (1938-39),  256;  James  F.  Nick- 
erson,  "Intonation  of  Solo  and  Ensemble  Performance  . . . ,"  Ibid.,  XXI  (1949), 
593-95. 

197 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


versity  of  Iowa1"  have  shown  that  there  is  no  such  thing  as  sta- 
bility of  pitch  among  singers:  scooping  is  found  in  almost  half 
the  attacks  and  averages  a  whole  tone  in  extent;  portamento  is 
very  common;  the  sustained  part  of  the  pitch  varies  from  the 
true  pitch  by  a  comma  or  more  in  one-fourth  of  the  notes  ana- 
lyzed. If  we  add  to  these  errors  the  omnipresent  vibrato,  with 
an  average  extent  of  a  semitone,  it  would  seem  that  the  ambitious 
and  optimistic  director  of  an  unaccompanied  choir  has  an  impos- 
sible task. 

Let  us  assume,  for  the  moment,  that  it  is  possible  for  a  choir 
to  sing  without  these  pitch  fluctuations,  that  all  its  members  can 
sing  a  note  a  comma  higher  or  lower  when  necessary,  and  that 
the  director  has  analyzed  the  music  and  marked  the  places  where 
the  comma  shifts  are  to  be  made.  What  have  we  then?  Strangely 
enough,  if  the  harmony  consists  of  simple  diatonic  progressions, 
typical  of  the  seventeenth  and  eighteenth  centuries,  the  pitch  will 
probably  falL  With  modal  progressions,  as  in  Palestrina,  it  is 
more  likely  to  remain  stationary.  According  to  GustavEngel,  if 
one  were  to  consider  possible  comma  shifts  whenever  a  modu- 
lation occurs,  most  of  the  recitatives  in  Mozart's  Don  Giovanni 
would  fall  from  one  to  four  commas  if  sung  unaccompanied,  and 
the  final  pitch  of  the  opera  would  be  five  or  six  semitones  flatter 
than  at  the  beginning,  A  or  A*3  instead  of  D! 

If  the  music  contains  much  chromaticism  and  remote  modu- 
lations, even  the  best-trained  choir  would  probably  flounder. 
And  yet  there  are  choral  compositions  of  the  sixteenth  and  early 
seventeenth  centuries  that  seem  strikingly  modern  because  of 
these  very  features.  De  Rore's  madrigal  "Calami  sonum  fer- 
entes"  for  four  basses  (c.  1555)  begins  with  an  ascending  chro- 
matic scale  passage  treated  in  imitation.  Later  it  has  a  re- 
markable faburden  of  inverted  major  triads  a  semitone  apart  — G 
F#  G  AD  G.  Caimo's  madrigal  "E  ben  raggion"  (1585)  contains 
a  very  smooth  example  of  modulation  in  which  the  F*  major  triad 
is  heard,  and,  24  bars  later,  its  enharmonic  equivalent,  the  G" 
major  triad.  In  just  intonation  the  latter  triad  would  be  a  large 
diesis  (42  cents,  or  almost  a  quarter  tone)  higher  than  the  former. 

16Carl  E„  Seashore,  The  Vibrato  (Iowa  City,  Iowa,  1932). 
198 


FROM  THEORY  TO  PRACTICE 


And  what  of  Marenzio's  madrigal  "O  voiche  sospirate  a  mig- 
liornote,''  where  there  is  a  modulation  around  the  circle  of  fifths 
from  C  to  G",  an  enharmonic  change  from  Gb  to  F#,  and  further 
modulation  on  the  sharp  side?  According  to  Kroyer,  from  whom 
all  these  examples  have  been  taken,  this  is  the  first  time  in  mu- 
sic that  the  circle  of  fifths  has  been  completed. *'  Could  Mar- 
enzio's madrigal  have  been  sung  in  just  intonation? 

Gesualdo  has  the  respect  of  the  moderns  because  of  his  har- 
monic freedom.  The  best  known  of  his  chromatic  madrigals  is 
the  "Resta  di  darmi  noia,"  in  which  he  passes  from  G  minor  to 
E  major,  and  then  sequentially  from  A  minor  to  F#  major.  Lis- 
ten to  the  recording  of  this  madrigal  by  a  group  of  unaccompa- 
nied singers  in  the  album  2000  Years  of  Music  and  you  will  prob- 
ably agree  that  the  attempt  to  record  it  was  a  noble  experiment 
and  nothing  more. 

Of  course  the  point  that  is  missed  by  all  these  rabid  expo- 
nents of  just  intonation  in  choral  music  is  that  this  music  was 
not  ordinarily  sung  unaccompanied  in  the  sixteenth  century.  A^ 
cappella  meant  simply  the  absence  of  independent  accompani- 
ment, not  of  all  accompaniment.  If  a  choir  usually  sang  motets 
accompanied  by  an  organ  in  meantone  temperament,  it  would 
quickly  adapt  itself  to  the  intonation  of  the  organ.  If  this  choir 
were  in  the  habit  of  singing  madrigals  accompanied  by  lutes  or 
viols  in  equal  temperament,  its  thirds  would  be  as  sharp  as  the 
thirds  are  today.  Kroyer  thought  the  pronounced  chromaticism 
of  the  Italian  madrigalists  showed  the  influence  of  keyboard  in- 
struments. On  the  contrary:  it  must  have  been  the  fretted  in- 
struments, already  in  equal  temperament,  that  influenced  com- 
posers like  de  Rore,  Caimo,  Marenzio,  and  Gesualdo  to  write 
passages  in  madrigals  that  could  not  have  been  sung  in  tune  with- 
out accompaniment. 


Present  Practice 

What  is  tuning  like  today?    A  generation  ago,  Anglas  made 
some  excellent  observations  about  the  intonation  of  the  symphony 

i  n 

1(Theodor  Kroyer,  "Die  Anfange  der  Chromatik  im  italienischen  Madrigal 
des  XVI.    Jahrhunderts,"  Internationale  Musikgesellschaft,  Beiheft  4  (1902). 

199 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


orchestra.  1°  The  pedals  of  the  harp  are  constructed  to  produce 
the  semitones  of  equal  temperament;  therefore,  once  the  harp  is 
put  in  tune  with  itself,  it,  and  it  alone  of  all  the  instruments,  will 
be  in  equal  temperament  The  violins  show  a  tendency  toward 
the  Pythagorean  tuning,  both  because  of  the  way  they  are  strung 
and  because  of  the  players'  tendency  to  play  sharps  higher  than 
enharmonic  flats.  Furthermore,  in  a  high  register  both  the  vio- 
lins and  the  flutes  are  likely  to  play  somewhat  sharp  for  the  sake 
of  brilliance.  He  might  have  added  that  the  brass  instruments, 
making  use  of  a  more  extended  portion  of  the  harmonic  series 
than  the  woodwinds,  have  a  natural  inclination  toward  just  into- 
nation in  certain  keys.  The  result  is  "a  very  great  lack  of  pre- 
cision," with  heterogeneous  sounds  that  are  a  mixture  of  "just, 
Pythagorean,  tempered,  or  simply  false."  Of  course  the  ears  of 
the  audience,  trained  for  years  to  endure  such  cacophony,  ac- 
tually are  pleased  by  what  seems  to  be  a  good  performance. 

LI.  S.  Lloyd  has  written  an  article  with  the  frightening  title 
"The  Myth  of  Equal  Temperament."1^  It  would  be  pretty  dis- 
couraging for  the  present  author  to  have  done  extended  research 
upon  the  history  of  equal  temperament  only  to  learn  at  last  that 
his  subject  matter  was  in  the  class  with  the  story  of  Cupid  and 
Psyche!  But  Lloyd  has  not  actually  consigned  equal  temperament 
to  the  category  of  the  tale  of  George  Washington  and  the  cherry 
tree.  His  argument  is  against  rigidity  of  intonation,  the  rigidity 
that  is  inherent  in  any  fixed  system  of  tuning.  He  holds  that  the 
players  in  a  string  quartet  or  the  singers  in  a  madrigal  group 
are  likely  to  be  guided  by  the  music  itself  as  to  what  intonation 
to  use,  sometimes  approaching  Pythagorean  intervals  when  me- 
lodic considerations  are  paramount  or  just  intervals  when  the 
harmony  demands  it.  And  undoubtedly  this  freedom  of  intonation, 
plus  a  well-defined  vibrato,  does  increase  the  charm  of  these 
more  intimate  chamber  ensembles. 

Not  even  the  piano  is  exempt  from  the  charge  of  inexactness. 
Three-quarters  of  a  century  ago  Alexander  Ellis  showed  that  the 

18J.  P.  L.  Anglas,    Precis    d'   acoustique    physique,  musical,  physiologique 
(Paris,  1910),  p.  206. 

19Music  and  Letters,  XXI  (1940),  347-361. 
200 


FROM  THEORY  TO  PRACTICE 


best  British  tuners  of  his  day  failed  to  tune  pianos  in  equal  tem- 
perament within  desirable  limits  of  error.  There  is  no  reason 
to  believe  that  modern  British  tuners,  or  American  ones  either, 
are  doing  a  better  job  than  was  done  then.  Schuck  and  Young 
even  show  that,  because  of  the  inharmonicity  of  the  upper  par- 
tials  of  the  piano,  a  tuner  is  bound  to  tune  the  upper  octaves  pro- 
gressively sharper  and  the  lowest  octaves  progressively  flatter 
than  those  in  the  middle  range. *®  Their  theoretical  findings 
agree  with  measurements  Railsback  had  already  made  of  pianos 
tuned  in  equal  temperament.  However,  the  psychologists  tell  us 
that  "stretched"  octaves  at  top  and  bottom  are  a  concomitant  of 
normal  hearing.  Therefore  the  sharpness  and  the  flatness  re- 
spectively would  probably  be  heard  as  correct  intonation. 

Now  all  of  this  paints  a  dismal  picture.  Apparently  nobody  — 
not  the  pianist,  nor  the  singer,  nor  the  violinist,  nor  the  wind- 
player  —  is  able  to  perform  in  correct  equal  temperament.  The 
harpist  is  left  sitting  alone,  but  no  doubt  he  will  be  joined  by  the 
Hammond  organist,  whose  instrument  comes  closest  to  the  equal 
tuning. 

This  contemporary  dispute  about  tuning  is  perhaps  a  tempest 
in  a  teapoto  It  is  probably  true  that  all  the  singers  and  players 
are  singing  and  playing  false  most  of  the  time.  But  their  errors 
are  errors  from  equal  temperament.  No  well-informed  person 
today  would  suggest  that  these  errors  consistently  resemble  de- 
partures from  just  intonation  or  from  any  other  tuning  system 
described  in  these  pages.  Equal  temperament  does  remain  the 
standard,  however  imperfect  the  actual  accomplishment  may  be. 

The  trend  of  musical  composition  during  the  late  nineteenth 
and  the  first  half  of  the  twentieth  century  has  been  to  exploit  the 
resources  of  equal  temperament,  of  an  octave  divided  into  12 
equal  parts,  and  hence  also  into  2,  3,  4,  or  6  parts.  To  ascertain 
how  far  back  this  trend  extends  is  not  the  purpose  of  this  book. 
It  would  be  foolish  to  deny  that  this  modern  trend  is  different  in 
kind  from  the  progressions  of  classic  harmony,  progressions 
that  were  almost  as  common  in  1600  as  in  1800.  But  it  may  be 
denied  that  these  classic  progressions  were  intimately  connected 

20O.  H.  Schuck  and  R.  W.  Young,  "Observations  on  the  Vibrations  of  Piano 
Strings,"  Journal  of  the  Acoustical  Society  of  America,  XV  (1943),  1-11. 


201 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


with  the  meantone  temperament,  as  has  often  been  alleged;  for 
we  have  seen  that  the  original  1/4 -comma  meantone  system  did 
not  even  reign  supreme  in  1600,  much  less  in  1700  or  1750.  In 
1600  there  were  half  a  dozen  or  more  ways  to  tune  the  octave; 
in  1732  Neidhardt  gave  his  readers  a  choice  of  twenty!  Moreover, 
there  is  every  reason  to  believe  that  in  practice  there  were  far 
greater  departures  from  these  extremely  varied  tuning  methods 
of  the  seventeenth  and  eighteenth  centuries  than  there  are  from 
equal  temperament  today. 

In  the  very  nature  of  things,  equal  temperament  has  undergone 
vicissitudes  during  the  last  four  hundred  years,  and  will  continue 
to  do  so.  Perhaps  the  philosophical  Neidhardt  should  be  allowed 
to  have  the  last  word  on  the  subject:  "Thus  equal  temperament 
carries  with  itself  its  comfort  and  discomfort,  like  the  holy  es- 
tate of  matrimony." 21 


^*Gantzlich  erschopfte,  mathematische  Abtheilungen,  p.  41. 
202 


LITERATURE  CITED 


LITERATURE  CITED 


Adlung,  Jacob.  Anleitung  zu  den  musikalischenGelahrtheit.    Er- 
furt, 1758. 
Musica  mechanica  organoedi.    Berlin,  1768. 

Agricola,  Martin.    Musica  instrumentalis  deudsch.    4th  ed.,  Wit- 
tenberg, 1545.    Reprinted  as  Band  20  of  Publikation 
alterer  praktischer  und  theoretischer  Musikwerke, 
1896. 
Rudimenta  musices.    Wittenberg,  1539. 

Amiot,  Pere  Joseph  Maria.  De  la  musique  des  ChinoiSo  (Me- 
moires  concernant  Thistoire,  .  .  ,  des  Chinois,  Vol. 
VI.)    Paris,  1780. 

Anglas,  Jules  Philippe  Louis.  Precis  d'acoustique  physique, 
musical,  physiologique.    Paris,  1910. 

Arieh  Das  Relativitatsprincip  der  musikalischen  Harmonie, 
Band  I.    Leipzig,  1925. 

Aristoxenus.  See  H.  S.  Macran,  The  Harmonics  of  Aristoxenus. 
Oxford,  1902. 

Aron,  PietrOo  Toscanello  in  musica.  Venice,  1523.  Revised 
edition  of  1529  was  consulted. 

Artusi,  Giovanni  Maria.  Seconda  parte  dell'  Artusi  overo  della 
imperfettioni  della  moderna  musica.    Venice,  1603. 

Awraamoff,  A.  M.  "Jenseits  von  Temperierung  und  Tonalitat," 
Melos,  I  (1920),  131-134,  160-166,  184-188. 

Bach,  Johann  Sebastian.  For  his  keyboard  works  see  particularly 
Vols.  Ill,  XIII,  XIV,  XV,  XXV,  and  XXXVIII  of  the 
Bach-Gesellschaft  Edition,  Leipzig,  1851-1900;  fac- 
simile edition,  Ann  Arbor,  1947. 

Bakfark,  Valentin.  (His  lute  music  is  reprinted  in  Denkmaler 
der  Tonkunst  in  Osterreich,  Vol.  XVHI.) 

Barbour,  J.  Murray.  "Bach  and  The  Art  of  Temperament, "  Mu- 
sical Quarterly,  XXXIII  (1947),  64-89. 

"Irregular  Systems  of  Temperament,"  Journal  of  the 

American  Musicological  Society,  I  (1948),  20-26. 

"Just  Intonation  Confuted,"  Music  and  Letters,  XIX 

(1938),  48-60. 


205 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


"Music  and  Ternary  Continued  Fractions,"  Ameri- 


can  Mathematical  Monthly,  LV  (1948),  545-555. 

"Musical    Logarithms,"    Scripta   Mathematica,    VII 

(1940),  21-31. 

"Nierop's  Hackebort,"  Musical  Quarterly,  XX  (1934), 

312-319. 

"The  Persistence  of  the  Pythagorean  Tuning  System," 

Scripta  Mathematica,  I  (1933),  286-304. 
"A  Sixteenth  Century  Approximation  for  TT,"  Amer- 
ican Mathematical  Monthly,  XL  (1933),  68-73~ 

Barca,  Alessandro.  "Introduzione  a  una  nuova  teoria  di  musica, 
memoria  prima,"  Accademia  di  scienze,  lettere  ed 
arti  in  Padova.  Saggi  scientifici  e  lettari  (Padova, 
1786),  pp.  365-418. 

Bartolus,  Abraham.  Musica  mathematica:  the  2nd  part  of  Hein- 
rich  Zeising's  Theatrimachinarum.  Altenburg,  1614. 

Bedos  (Dom  Franqois  Bedos  de  Celles).  L' art  du  f acteur  d' orgues , 
2nd  part.    Paris,  1770.    Facsimile  ed.,  Kassel,  1935. 

Beer,  Johann.  Schola  phonologica:  a  manuscript  work  referred 
to  in  Mattheson's  Critica  musica. 

Bendeler,  Johann  Philipp.  Organopoeia.  2nd  ed.,  Frankfurt  and 
Leipzig,  1739. 

Berlin,  Johann  Daniel.  Anleitung  zur  Tonometrie.  Copenhagen 
and  Leipzig,  1767. 

Bermudo,  Juan.  Declaracion  de  instrumentos  musicales.  Os- 
suna,  1555. 

Blankenburg,  Quirinus  van.    Elementa  musica.    The  Hague,  1739. 

Bohme,  Franz  Magnus.  Geschichte  des  Tanzes  in  Deutschland. 
Leipzig,  1886. 

Boethius,  A.  M.  S.  De  institutione  musica.  German  translation 
by  Oscar  Paul.    Leipzig,  1872. 

Bonnet,  Joseph.  Historical  Organ  Recitals,  Vol.  I.  New  York, 
c.  1917. 

Bosanquet,  R.  H.  M.    An  Elementary  Treatise  on  Musical  Inter- 
vals and  Temperament.    London,  1876. 
"On  the  Hindoo  Division  of  the  Octave,  Royal  Soci- 
ety's Proceedings  (1877),  pp.  372-384. 


206 


LITERATURE  CITED 


"Temperament;  or,  the  Division  of  the  Octave," 
Proceedings  of  the  Musical  Association  (1874-75), 
pp.  4-17. 


Bossier,  Heinrich  Philipp.  Elementarbuch  der  Tonkunst.  Speier, 

1782. 
Bottrigari,  Ercole.    II  desiderio,  owero  de*   concerti  di  varii 

stromenti  musicali.  Venice,  1594.    New  ed.  byKathi 

Meyer,  Berlin,  1924. 
Buttstett,  Johann  Heinrich.    Kurze  Anfuhrung  zum  General-Bass. 

2nd  ed.    Leipzig,  1733. 
Caramuel  (Juan  Caramuel  de  Lobkowitz),  Mathesis  nova.  Cam- 
pania, 1670. 
Cardano,  Girolamo.    Opera  omnia,  ed.     Sponius.     Lyons,   1663. 
Caus,  Salomon  de.    Les  raisons  des  forces  mouvantes  avec  di- 

verses  machines,    Francfort,  1615. 
Cavazzoni,  Girolamo.    Composizioni.    Reprinted   in   I  classici 

della  musica  italiana,  Vol.  VI.    Milan,  1919. 
Choquel,  Henri  Louis.    La  musique  rendue  sensible  par  la  m4- 

chanique.   New  ed.,  Paris,  1762. 
Colonna,  Fabio.    La  sambuca  lincea.   Naples,  1618. 
Couperin,  Frangois.    Pieces  d'orgue.    Paris,  1690.    Reprinted 

as  Tome  VI,  Oeuvres  completes,  Paris,  1932-33. 
Courant,  Maurice.    "Chine  etCoree,"  Encyclopedic  de  la  musique 

et dictionnaire du  conservatoire  (Paris,  1913),  Parti, 

Vol.  I,  pp.  77-241. 
Crotch,  William.    Elements  of  Musical  Composition.    London, 

1812. 
Dechales,  R.  P.    Claudius  Franciscus  Milliet.    Cursus  seu  mun- 

dus  mathematicus,  Tomus  Tertius.    Lugduni,  1674. 
Delezenne,  Charles  Edouard  Joseph.    "Memoire  sur  les  valeurs 

numeriques  des  notes  de  la  gamme,"    Recueil  des 

travaux  de  la  societe  des  sciences,  .  .  .  ,  de  Lille 

(1826-27),  pp.  1-56. 
Denis,  Jean.    Traite  de  V accord  de  Tespinette.    Paris,  1650. 
Doni,  Giovanni  Battista.    Compendio  del  trattato  de*   generi,  e 

de'  modi.    Rome,  1635. 


207 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Douwes,  Claas.  Grondig  Ondersoek  van  de  Toonen  der  Musijk. 
Franeker,  1699. 

Dowland,  Robert.    Variety  of  Lute -Lessons.    London,  1610. 

Drobisch,  M.  W.  "Uber  musikalische  Tonbestimmungund  Tem- 
per atur,"  Abhandlungen  der  mathematisch-physi- 
schenClasse  der  koniglich  sachsischen  Gesellschaft 
der  Wissenschaften,  IV  (1855),  1-120. 

Dupont,  Wilhelm.  Geschichte  der  musicalischen  Temperatur. 
Erlangen,  1935. 

Durante,  Francesco.  Sonate,  toccate  e  divertimenti.  Reprinted 
in  I  classici  della  musica  italiana,  Vol.  XI.  Milan, 
1919. 

Ellis,  Alexander  J.  "On  the  History  of  Musical  Pitch,"  Journal 
of  Society  of  Arts,  XXVIII  (1880),  293-336. 

"On  the  Musical  Scales  of  Various  Nations,"  Journal 

of  Society  of  Arts,  XXXffl  (1885),  485-527. 

Engel,  Gustav.  "Eine  mathematisch-harmonische  Analyse  des 
Don  Giovanni  von  Mozart,"  Vierteljahrsschrift  fur 
Musikwissenschaft,  III  (1887),  491-560. 

Euler,  Leonard.  Tentamen  novae  theoriae  musicae.  St.  Peters- 
burg, 1739. 

Exposition  de  quelques  nouvelles  vues  mathematiques 

dans  la  theorie  de  la  musique.    Amsterdam,  1760. 

Faber  Stapulensis.   Elementa  musicalia.    Paris,  1496. 

Faggot,  Jacob.    See  p.  65,  note  43,  of  this  study. 

Farey,  John.  "On  a  New  Mode  of  Equally  Tempering  the  Musi- 
cal Scale,"  Philosophical  Magazine,  XXVII  (1807), 
65-66. 

"On  Music,"   Philosophical  Magazine,  XXVI  (1806), 

171-176. 

Fetis,  Frangois  Joseph.  Histoire  gerie'rale  de  la  musique.  5  vols. 
Paris,  1869-76. 

Fischer,  Johann  Philip  Albrecht.  Verhandlung  van  de  Klokken 
en  het  Klokke-SpeL    Utrecht,  1738. 

Faulhaber,  Johann.  Ingenieurschul.  Frankfort  am  Mayn,  1630, 
(Reference  in  Neidhardt,  Sectio  canonis  harmonici.) 


208 


LITERATURE  CITED 


Fludd,  Robert.    Sophiae  cum  moria  certamen.    Frankfort,  1629. 
Fogliano,  Lodovico.    Musica  theorica.    Venice,  1529. 
Frescobaldi,  Girolamo.     Composizioni  per  organo  e  cembalo. 

Reprinted  in  I  classici  della  musica  italiana,  Vol. 

XIL    Milan,  1919. 
Fritz,  Bartholdo    Anweisung  wie  man  Clavier e,  Clavicins,  und 

Orgeln,  nach  einer  mechanischen  Art,  in  alien  zwblf 

Tbnen  gleich  rein  stimmen  konne, . . .  3rd  ed.,  Leip- 

Froberger,  Johann  Jakob.     (His  clavier  compositions  are  re- 
printed in  Denkmaler  der  Tonkunst  in  Osterreich, 

Vol.  VI,  Part  2,  and  Vol.  X,  Part  2.) 
Fuller-Maitland,  J.  A.,  and  W.  Barclay  Squire.    The  Fitzwilliam 

Virginal  Book,  Vol.  I.    Leipzig,  1899. 
Gafurius,  Franchinus.    Practica  musica.    Milan,  1496. 
Galilei,  Galileo.    Discorsi  e  dimostrazioni  matematiche  intorno 

e  due  nuove  scienze.    Leyden,  1638. 
Galilei,    Vincenzo.     Dialogo   della   musica   antica   e   moderna. 

Florence,  1581. 
Fronimo,  dialogo  sopra  Tarte  del  bene  intavolare  e 

rettamente  suonare  la  musica  . . .  nel  liuto.     1st  ed., 

Venice,  1581;  revised  ed.,  1584. 
Galin,  Pierre.    Exposition  d'une   nouvelle    m^thode  pour  l'en- 

seignement  de  la  musique.    1st  ed.,  1818;   3rd  ed., 

Bordeaux  and  Paris,  1862. 
Gallimard,  Jean  Edme.    h'  arithmetique  des  musiciens.    Paris, 

1754. 
Ganassi,  Sylvestro.   Regola  Rubertina.    Lettione  seconda,  1543; 

ed.  Max  Schneider,  Leipzig,  1924. 
Garnault,  Paul.    Le  temperament,  son  histoire,  son  application 

aux  claviers,  aux  violes  de  gambe  et  guitares,  son 

influence  surla  musique  du  xviiiesiecle.   Nice,  1929. 
Gesualdo  (Carlo  Gesualdo  da  Venosa).   Madrigale.   Reprinted  in 

I  classici  della  musica  italiana,  Vol.  XIV.    Milan, 

1919. 
Gibbons,  Orlando.    Complete  Keyboard  Works,  ed.  Margaret  H. 

Glyn.    5  vols,  in  1.    London,  1925. 


209 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Gibelius,  Otto.    Propositiones  mathematico-musicae.    Mlinden, 

1666. 
Gintzler,  Simon.    Intabolatura  de  lauto.  Venice,  1547.    Reprinted 

in  Denkmaler  der  Tonkunst  in  Osterreich,  Vol.  XVIII. 
Glyn,  Margaret  H.    Early  English  Organ  Music.    London,  1939. 
Gow,  James.   A  Short  History  of  Greek  Mathematics .  Cambridge, 

1884.   Reprinted,  New  York,  1923. 
Grammateus,  Henricus.    Ayn  new  kunstlich  Buech.    Nlirnberg, 

1518. 
Greene,  Paul  C.    "Violin  Intonation,"   Journal  of  the  Acoustical 

Society  of  America,  IX  (1937-38),  43-44. 
Haser,  A.   F\    "Uber  wissenschaftliche  Begriindung  der  Musik 

durch  Akustik,"    Allgemeine  musikalische   Zeitung, 

Vol.  XXXH  (1829),  cols.  143-147. 
Hammond,  Laurens.    United  States  Patent,  1,956,350,  April  24, 

1934. 
Helmholtz,  H.  L.  F.    Sensations  of  Tone.    2nd  English  ed.,  trans- 
lated by  Alexander  J.  Ellis.    London,  1885. 
Henfling,  Konrad.    "Specimen  de  novo  suo  systemate  musico." 

Abhandlungen  der  Berliner  Akademie,  1710.    (Ref- 

erence  in  Sauveur,  "Table  ge'ne'rale  . . . .") 
Hizler,  Daniel.    Extract  aus  der  neuen  Musica  oder  Singkunst. 

Nurnberg,  1623. 
Holder,  William.  Treatise  . . .  of  Harmony.  3rd  ed.,  London,  1731. 
Hugo  de  Reutlingen.    Flores  musicae  omnis  cantus  Gregoriani. 

Strassburg,  1488. 
Hutton,  Charles.    Mathematical  Dictionary.    New  ed.,  London, 

1815. 
Huyghens,  Christian.     "Novus    cyclus   harmonious, "    in   Opera 

varia.    Leyden,  1724. 
Jackson,  William.    A  Scheme  Demonstrating  the  Perfection  and 

Harmony  of  Sounds.    London,  1726  (?). 
Janko,  Paul  von. "  Uber  mehr  als  zwolfstufige  gleichschwebende 

Temperaturen,"  Beitrage  zur  Akustik   und   Musik- 

wissenschaft  (1901),  pp.  6-12. 
Jeans,  Sir  James.    Science  and  Music.    New  York,  1937. 


210 


LITERATURE  CITED 


Judenkiinig,  Hans.    Ain  schone  kunstliche  Underweisung.  Vienna, 

1523.    Reprinted  in  Denkmaler  der  Tonkunst  in  6s- 

sterreich,  Vol.  XVUI. 
Keller,  Godfrey.    A  Compleat  Method . . .  London,  before  1707. 

His  tuning  rules  were  also  printed  in  William  Hol- 
der's Treatise. 
Kepler,  Johannes.    Harmonices    mundi.    Augsburg,   1619.     Ed. 

Ch.  Frisch,  Frankfort  am  Main,  1864. 
Kinkeldey,  Otto.    Orgel  und  Klavier  in  der  Musik  des  16.    Jahr- 

hunderts.    Leipzig,  1910. 
Kircher,  Athanasius.    Musurgia  universalis,  Vol.  I.    Rome,  1650. 
Kirnberger,  Johann  Philipp.    Die  Kunst  des  reinen  Satzes  in  der 

Musik,  2nd  part,  3rd  division.    Berlin,  1779. 
Korte,  Oskar.    "Laute  und  Lautenmusik  bis  zur  Mitte  des  16. 

Jahrhunderts,"  Beiheft  3,  Internationale  Musikgesell- 

schaft,  1901. 
Kornerup,  Thorvald.    Das  goldene  Tonsystem  als  Fundament  der 

theoretischen  Akustik.    Copenhagen,  1935. 
Das  Tonsystem  des  Italieners  Zarlino.  Copenhagen, 

1930. 
Kroyer,  Theodor.    "Die  Anfange  der  Chromatik  im  italienischen 

Madrigal  des  XVI.    Jahrhunderts,"  Beiheft  4,  Inter- 
nationale Musikgesellschaft,  1902. 
Kuhnau,  Johann.     (His    clavier  compositions   are  reprinted   in 

Denkmaler  deutscher  Tonkunst,  IV  Band,  1.    Folge.) 
La  Laurencie,  Lionel  de.    Le  violon  de  Lully  a  Viotti,  Tome  HI. 

Paris,  1924. 
Lambert,  Johann  Heinrich.    "Remarques  sur  les  temperaments 

en  musique,"  Memoirs  of  the  Berlin  Academy,  1774. 

German  translation  in  Historisch-Kritische  Beytrage 

zur  Aufnahme  der  Musik,  V  (1760-78),  417-50. 
Lanfranco,  Giovanni  Maria.    Scintillede  musica.    Brescia,  1533. 
Levens.    Abr^ge*  des  regies  de  l'harmonie.    Bordeaux,  1743. 
Liston,  Henry.    An  Essay  upon  Perfect  Intonation.    Edinburgh, 

1812, 
Lloyd,  Llewellyn  S.    "The  Myth  of  Equal  Temperament,"  Music 

and  Letters,  XXI  (1940),  347-361. 


211 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Malcolm,  Alexander.    A  Treatise  of  Musick.    Edinburgh,  1721. 
Marinati,  Aurelio.    Somma  di  tutte  le  scienza.    Rome,  1587. 
Marpurg,  Friedrich  Wilhelm.    Versuch  liber  die  musikalische 

Temperatur.    Breslau,  1776. 
Mattheson,  Johann.    Critica  musica,  Vol.  II.    Hamburg,  1722. 
Meckenheuser,  Jakob  Georg,    Die  sogenannte  allerneueste  mus- 

icalische  Temperatur.    Quedlinburg,  1727. 
Mercadier  (Jean  Baptiste  Mercadier  de  Belesta).    Nouveau  sys- 

temede  musique  theorique  et  pratique.    Paris,  1777. 

(Reference  in  Suremain-Missery.) 
Mercator,  Nicholas.    (A  manuscript  reference  to  him  is  given  in 

Holder's  Treatise.) 
Merian,  Wilhelm.    Per  Tanz  in  den  deutschen  Tabulaturbiichern. 

Leipzig,  1927. 
Mersenne,  Marin.    Cogitata  physico-mathematica.    Paris,  1644. 

Harmonicorum  libri  XII.    Paris,  1648. 

Harmonie  universelle.    Paris,  1636-37. 


Metius,  Adrian.  (There  is  an  inexplicit  reference  to  him  in 
Nierop's  Wis-konstige  Musyka.) 

Montucla,  Jean  Etienne.  Histoire  des  mathematiques .  New  ed., 
Paris,  1802.    Vol.  IV. 

Montvallon,  Andre  Barrigue  de.  Nouveau  systeme  de  musique 
sur  les  intervalles  des  tons  et  sur  les  proportions 
des  accords,  Aix,  1742.  (Reference  in  Romieu's 
_ Memoir e  theorique  &  pratique. ") 

Morley,  Thomas.  A  Plaine  and  Easie  Introduction  to  Practicall 
Musicke.    London,  1597. 

Nassarre,  Pablo.    Escuela  musica,  Part  I.    Zaragoza,  1724. 

Naylor,  Edward  Woodall.  An  Elizabethan  Virginal  Book.  Lon- 
don, 1905. 

Neidhardt,  Johann  George.  Beste  und  leichteste  Temperatur  des 
Monochordi.    Jena,  1706. 

Gantzlich   erschopfte,  mathematische   Abtheilungen 

des  diatonisch-chromatischen,  temperirten  Canonis 
Monochordi.    Konigsberg  and  Leipzig,  1732. 
Sectio  canonis  harmonici.    Konigsberg,  1724. 


212 


LITERATURE  CITED 


Nickerson,  James  F.  "Intonation  of  Solo  and  Ensemble  Perform- 
ance . . . ,"  Journal  of  the  Acoustical  Society  of  Amer- 
ica, XXI  (1949),  593-595. 

Nierop,  Dyrk  Rembrantz  van.  Wis-konstige  Musyka.  Amster- 
dam, 1650.    2nd  ed.,  1659. 

Norden,  N.  Lindsay.  "A  New  Theory  of  Untempered  Music,"  Mu- 
sical Quarterly,  XXII  (1936),  217-233. 

Opelt,  Friedrich  Wilhelm.  Allgemeine  Theorie  der  Musik.  Leip- 
zig, 1852. 

Ornithoparchus,  Andreas.  Musicae  activaemicrologus.  Leipzig, 
1517.  English  translation  by  John  Dowland,  London, 
1609. 

Pachelbel,  Johann.  (His  clavier  compositions  are  reprinted  in 
Denkmaler  der  Tonkunst  in  Bayern,  II  Jahrgang,  1. 
Band,  and  his  organ  compositions  in  Ibid.,  IV  Jahr- 
gang, 1.    Band) 

Parthenia  or  the  Maydenhead  of  the  First  Musicke  that  ever  was 
Printed  for  the  Virginalls.    London,  1611. 

Planck,  Max.  "Die  naturliche  Stimmung  in  der  modernen  Vokal- 
musik,"  Vierteljahrsschrift  fiir  Musikwissenschaft, 
LX  (1893),  418-440. 

Poole,  Henry  Ward,  "On  Perfect  Harmony  in  Music  . . .",  Silliman's 
American  Journal  of  Science  and  Arts,  2nd  Series, 
XXXXIV  (1867),  1-22. 
-"On  Perfect  Musical  Intonation,"  Silliman's  Ameri- 


can  Journal  of  Science  and  Arts,  LX  (1850),  68-83, 

199-216. 
Praetorius,  Michael.    Syntagma  musicum,  Vol.  n.    Wolfenblittel, 

1618.    New  edition  published  as  13.    Band,  Publika- 

tion  alterer  praktischer  und  theoretischer  Musik- 

werke,  1884. 
Prelleur,  Pierre.    The  Compleat  Tutor  for  the  Harpsichord  or 

Spinet  (Modern  Musick-Master,  Part  VI).    London, 

c.    1730. 


213 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Printz,  Wolff  gang  Caspar.  Phrynis  Mytilenaeus  oder  der  saty- 
rische  Componist.    Dresden  and  Leipzig,  1696. 

"Pro  clavichordiis  faciendis.  "A  fifteenth-century  Erlangen  man- 
uscript described  in  Dupont's  Geschichte. 

Ptolemy,  Claudius.  Harmonicorum  libri  tres.  Latin  translation 
by  John  Wallis.    London,  1699. 

Rameau,  Jean  Philippe.    Generation  harmonique.    Paris,  1737. 

Nouveau  syst&mede  musique  theorique.  Paris,  1726. 

Traite  de  P harmonic    Paris,  1722. 

Ramis  (Bartolomeus  Ramis  de  Pareja).  Musica  practica.  Bo- 
logna, 1482.  New  edition,  by  Johannes  Wolf,  pub- 
lished as  a  Beiheft  of  the  Internationale  Musikgesell- 
schaft,  1901. 

Rees,  Abraham.  New  Cyclopedia.  45  vols.  London,  1802-20. 
The  reference  in  the  present  study  is  to  Vol.  XIV  of 
first  American  edition. 

Reinhard,  Andreas.    Monochordum.    Leipzig,  1604. 

Riemann,  Hugo.    Geschichte  der  Musiktheorie.    Berlin,  1898. 
Populare  Darstellung  der  Akustik.    Berlin,  1896. 

Robet-Maugin,  J.  C.  Manuel  du  Luthier.  Paris,  1834.  (Reference 
in  Garnault's  Le  temperament.) 

Romberg,  Bernard.    Violoncell  Schull.    Berlin,  1840  (?)• 

Romieu,  Jean  Baptiste  (?).  "Memoire  theorique  &  pratique  sur 
les  syst^mes  temperas  de  musique,"  Me'moires  de 
l'acade'mie  royale  des  sciences  (1758),  pp.  805-870. 

Rossi,  Lemme.    Sistema  musico.    Perugia,  1666. 

Rousseau,  Jean,    Traite*  de  la  viole.    Paris,  1687. 

Rousseau,  Jean  Jaques.  Dictionnaire  de  musique.  Paris,  1768. 
(Reference  in  Opelf  s  Allgemeine  Theor ie  der  Musik.) 

Sabbatini,  Galeazzo.  Regola  facile  e  breve  per  sonare  sopra  il 
basso  continuo.  Venice,  1628.  (Reference  in  Kir- 
cher's  Musurgia  universalis.) 

Sachs,  Curt  (ed.).  Two  Thousand  iears  of  Music  (record album). 
London,  1931. 

Salinas,  FranciscOo    De  musica  libri  VII.    Salamanca,  1577. 

Sancta  Maria,  Tomas  de.  Arte  de  taner  fantasia.  Valladolid, 
1565. 


214 


LITERATURE  CITED 


Sauveur,  Joseph.    "Systeme  general  des  intervalles  des  sons," 

Me'moires  de  Tacade'mie  royale  des  sciences  (1701), 

pp.  403-498. 
"Table  geWrale  des  systemes  tempe're's  de  musique," 

Me'moires  de  l'acade'mie  royale  des  sciences  (1711), 

pp.  406-417. 
Scarlatti,  Domenico.    Klavierwerke,  ed.    Heinrich  Barth.  4  vols. 

Vienna,  c.    1901. 
Scheidt,  Samuel.    Tabulatura  nova.    3  vols.    Hamburg,  1624.  Re- 
printed in  Denkm'aler  deutscher  Tonkunst,  1.    Band. 
Schering,  Arnold.    Alte  Meister  aus  der  Frlihzeit  des  Orgelspiels. 

Leipzig,  1913. 
Schlick,  Arnold.     Tablaturen   etlicher  Lobgesang  und   Lidlein. 

Maintz,  1512.  Reprinted  in  Monatshefte  fur  Musik- 

geschichte,  I  (1869). 
Spiegel  der  Orgelmacher  und  Organisten.    Maintz . 

1511.    Reprinted  in  Monatshefte  fur  Musikgeschichte, 

I  (1869). 


Schneegass,  Cyriac.     Nova  &  exquisita  monochordi   dimensio. 

Erfurt,  1590. 
Schroter,  Christoph  Gottlieb.    "EinSendschreiben  liber  Temper- 

atur-Berechnung."    In  Marpurg's  Versuch. 
Schuck,  O.  H.,  and  R.  W.  Young.    "Observations  on  the  Vibrations 

of  Piano  Strings,"  Journal  of  the  Acoustical  Society 

of  America,  XV  (1943),  1-11. 
Schumann,  Karl  Erich.    Akustik.    Breslau,  1925. 
Seashore,  Carl  E.    The  Vibrato.    Iowa  City,  Iowa,  1932. 
Serini,  Giuseppe.    Sonate  per  cembalo.    Reprinted  in  I  classici 

della  musica  italiana,  Vol.  XXEX.   Milan,  1919. 
Small,  Arnold  M.    "Present-Day  Preferences  for  Certain  Me- 
lodic Intervals,"  Journal  of  the  Acoustical  Society  of 

America,  X  (1938-39),  256. 
Smith,  Robert.    Harmonics,  or  the  Philosophy  of  Musical  Sounds. 

Cambridge,  1749. 
Sorge,  Georg  Andreas.    GesprSch  zwischen  einem  Musico  the- 

oretico   und  einem  Studioso   musices.    Lobenstein, 

1748. 


215 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Spitta,  Philipp.  Johann  Sebastian  Bach.  2  vols.  Translated  by 
Clara  Bell  and  J.  A.  Fuller-Maitland.  London,  1884. 
Vol.  I. 

Stanhope,  Charles,  Earl.  "Principles  of  the  Science  of  Tuning  In- 
struments with  Fixed  Tones,"  Philosophical  Maga- 
zine, XXV  (1806),  291-312. 

Stevin,  Simon.  Van  de  Spiegeling  der  Singconst.  A  manuscript 
work  (c.  1600)  edited  by  D.  Bierens  de  Haan.  Am- 
sterdam, 1884. 

Str'ahle,  Daniel  P.  "Nytt  pafund,  til  at  finna  temperaturen,  i 
stamningen  for  thonerne  pa  claveretock  dylika  in- 
strumenter,"  Proceedings  of  the  Swedish  Academy, 
IV  (1743),  281-291. 

Sulzer,  Johann  Georg.  Allgemeine  Theorie  der  schbnen  Klinste. 
Leipzig,  1777-79. 

Suremain-Missery,  Antoine.  The'orie  acoustico-musicale.  Pa- 
ris, 1793. 

Sweelinck,  Jan  Pieters.  (His  compositions  for  organ  and  clavier 
are  in  Denkmaler  deutscher  Tonkunst,  IV  Band,  1. 
Folge.) 

Tanaka,  Shohe\  "Studien  im  Gebiete  der  reinen  Stimmung." 
Vierteljahrsschrift  fur  Musikwissenschaft,  VI  (1890), 
1-90. 

Telemann,  Georg  Philipp.  Drei  Dutzend  Klavier-Fantasien,  ed. 
MaxSeiffert.    3rd  ed.,  Kassel,  1935. 

Tempelhof,  Georg  Friedrich.  Gedanken  iiber  die  Temperatur 
des  Herrn  Kirnberger.   Berlin  and  Leipzig,  1775. 

Thompson,  Gen.  Perronet.  On  the  Principles  and  Practice  of 
Just  Intonation.  9th  ed.,  1866.  (Reference  in  Helm- 
holtz.) 

Torchi,  Luigi.  L'arte  musicale  in  Italia,  Vol.  III.  Milan,  post 
1897. 

Varella,  Domingos  de  S.  Jose.  Compendio  de  musica.  Porto, 
1806. 

Verheijen,  Abraham.  See  Stevin' s  Van  de  Spiegeling  der  Sing- 
const. 


216 


LITERATURE  CITED 


Vicentino,  Nicola.  L'antica  musica  ridotta  alia  moderna  prat- 
tica.    Rome,  1555. 

Warren,  Ambrose.    The  Tonometer.    London,  1725. 

Wasielewski,  Joseph  Wilhelm  von.  Geschichte  der  Instrumental  - 
musik  im  16.    Jahrhundert.    Berlin,  1878. 

Werckmeister,  Andreas.  Hypomnemata  musica.  Quedlinburg, 
1697. 

Musicalische    Paradoxal-Discourse.      Quedlinburg, 

1707. 

Musicalische  Temperatur.    Frankfort  and  Leipzig, 

1691. 

White,  William  Braid.  Piano  Tuning  and  Allied  Arts.  4th  ed., 
Boston,  1943. 

Wiese,  Christian  Ludwig  Gustav,  Baron  von.  Klangeintheilungs- , 
Stimmungs-  und  Temperatur-Lehre.  Dresden,  1793. 

Williamson,  Charles.  "  Frequency  Ratios  of  the  Tempered  Scale," 
Journal  of  the  Acoustical  Society  of  America,  X 
(1938),  135-136. 

Yasser,  Joseph.    A  Theory  of  Evolving  Tonality.  New  York,  1932. 

Young,  Thomas.  "Outlines  of  Experiments  and  Inquiries  Re- 
specting Sound  and  Light,"  Philosophical  Transac- 
tions, XC  (1800),  106-150. 

Zacconi,  LodovicOo    Prattica  di  musica,  Part  I.    Venice,  1592. 

Zarlino,  Gioseffo.    Dimostrationi  armoniche.    Venice,  1571. 

Istitutioni  armoniche.    Venice,  1558. 

Sopplimenti  musicali.   Venice,  1588. 

Zeising,  Heinrich.    Theatri  machinarum.    Altenburg,  1614. 

Zipoli,  Domenico.  Composizioni  per  organo  e  cembalo.  Re- 
printed in  I  classici  della  musica  italiana,  Vol. 
XXXVI.    Milan,  1919. 


217 


INDEX 


abacus  Triharmonicus,  109  f. 

Abrege  des  regies  de  l'harmonie.  See  Levens. 

Adlung,  J.,  85. 

Agricola,  M.,  4,  10,  95,  149-151. 

Akustik.    See  C.  E.  Schumann. 

Alexander  the  Great,  122. 

Allgemeine   Theorie   der   Musik.    See    F.    W. 

Opelt. 
Allgemeine  Theorie  der  Schonen  Kiinste.    See 

J.  G.  Sulzer. 
Amiot,  J.  M.,  77. 
"Die  Anfange  der  Chromatik  im  italienischen 

Madrigal  des  XVI.    Jahrhunderts."    See  T. 

Kroyer. 
Anglas,  J.  P.  L.,  197  f. 
Anleitung    zu  den    musikalischen  Gelahrtheit. 

See  J.  Adlung. 
Anleitung  zur  Tonometrie.    See  J.  D.  Berlin. 
Anonymous,  author  of  Exposition  de  quelques 

nouvelles  vues  mathematiques,  125. 
Anonymous,  author  of  Pro    clavichordiis   fa- 

ciendis,  91  f. 
L'antica  musica  ridotta  alia  modernaprattica. 

See  N.  Vicentino. 
Anweisung     wie    man    Claviere    ...    stimmen 

konne .    See  B.  Fritz, 
approximations    to    equal   temperament.     See 

temperament,      equal:      geometrical     and 

mechanical  approximations,  and  numerical 

approximations . 
approximations  to  the  meantone  temperament. 

See   temperament,    meantone:    approxima- 
tions. 
Appun,  G.,  119. 
Arabian  scale.    See   multiple   division:    equal 

divisions:    17-division. 
Archicembalo,  27,  115  f,  152. 
Archicymbalam,  106. 
Archimedes,  34,  50. 
Archytas,  16  f,  19,  22  f,  143. 
Ariel,  113. 

Aristoxenus,  2,  16  f,  19,  22-24,  57. 
arithmetical  division.    See  division,  arithmet- 
ical. 
L'arithmetique    des    musiciens.     See    J.    E. 

Gallimard. 
Aron,  P.,  10,  26,  49. 
L'art  du  facteur  d'orgues.  See  F.  Bedos. 
Arte    de   taner    fantasia.    See   T.    de    Sancta 

Maria. 
L'arte  musicale  in  Italia.  See  L.  Torchi. 
Artusi,  G.  M.,  8,  10,  46,_IT2,  144-148,  176, 

186. 
Awraamoff,  A.  M.,  24,  152. 

"Bach  and  The  Art  of  Temperament."    See  J. 

M.  Barbour. 
Bach,  C.  P.  E.,  47  f. 


Bach,  J.  N.,  85-87. 

Bach,  J.  S.,  10,  12  f,  85-87,  146,  189,  192-194. 

Bakfark,  V.,  185. 

Ballet  comique  de  la  reine,  8. 

Barbour,  J.  M.,  3,  77,  112,  131,  161,  189-197. 

Barca,  A.,  42  f. 

Bartolus,  A.,  142  f. 

Beaugrand,  J.  de,  79,  81,  84. 

Bedos,  F-,  125. 

Beer,  J.,  124. 

bells,  7,  142. 

Bendeler,  J.  P.,  157-160. 

Berlin,  J.  D.,  119. 

Bermudo,  J.,  3,  5,  46,  137,  162-164,  186. 

Bertelsen,  N.  P.  J.,  127. 

Beste  und  leichteste  Temperatur  des    Mono- 

chordi.    See  J.  G.  Neidhardt. 
Blankenburg,  Q.  van,  105  f,  118. 
Bohme,  F.  M.,  187. 
Boethius,  A.  M.  S.,  3,  121. 
bonded  clavichord.    See  clavichord,  bonded. 
Bonnet,  J.,  186. 
Bosanquet,  R.  H.  M.,  9,  32,  114,  117,  119-121, 

123,  125-127,  129  f,  131. 
Bossier,  H.  P.,  49  f. 
Bottrigari,  E.,  8,  46,  147  f. 
Boulliau,  I.,  54,  79-81. 
Bumler,  G.  H.,  80. 
Bull,  J.,  187  f,  192  f. 
Buttstett,  J.  H.,  105. 
Byrd,  W.,  188. 

Cahill,  T.,  74. 

Caimo,  J.,  196  f. 

Caramuel,  J.,  3. 

Cardano,  G.,  57. 

Caus,  S.  de,  11,  95  f,  101. 

cembalo.    See  keyboard  instruments. 

cent,  ii  and  passim. 

Cerone,  P.,  46. 

Cherubini,  M.  L-,  58. 

China,  7,  55  f,  77-79,  122,  150  f. 

"Chine  et  Coree."    See  M.  Courant. 

Choquel,  H.  L.,  152. 

choral  music.  See  just  intonation  in  choral 
music. 

chromatic  genius.    See  Greek  tunings. 

chromaticism,  187-189,  196  f. 

circle  of  fifths,  106,  188-194,  197. 

circulating  temperaments.  See  irregular  sys- 
tems:   circulating  temperaments. 

clavichord.    See  keyboard  instruments. 

,  bonded,  30  f,  145-147. 

clavier.   See  keyboard  instruments. 

closed  system.    See  temperament,  regular. 

Cogitata  physico-mathematica.  See  M.  Mer- 
senne. 

Colonna,  F.,  23  f,  151-154. 


219 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


column  of  differences.  See  tabular  differ- 
ences. 

comma,  i  and  passim.  See  especially  irregu- 
lar systems:    divisions  of  ditonic  comma. 

Compendio  de  musica.    See  D.  Varella. 

Compendio  del  trattato  de'  generi,  e  de'  modi- 
See  G.  B.  Doni. 

A  Compleat  Method 


See  G.  Keller. 


The  Compleat  Tutor  for  the  Harpsichord  or 
Spinet.    See  P.  Prelleur. 

continued  fractions,  54,  74,  124,  128-130. 

correspondences  between  equal  multiple  divi- 
sions and  varieties  of  meantone  tempera- 
ment, 124. 

Couperin,  F.,  191  f. 

Courant,  M.,  55  f,  122,  150. 

Critica  musica.    See  J.  Mattheson. 

Crotch,  W.,  31  f. 

cube  root.  See  duplication  of  the  cube;  loga- 
rithms; mesolabium. 

Cursus  seu  mundus  mathematicus.  See  R.  P. 
C.  F.  M.  Dechales. 


De  institutione  musica.  See  A.  M.  S.  Boethius. 
De  la  musique  des  Chinois.    See  J.  M.  Amiot. 
De  musica  libri  VII.    See  F.  Salinas, 
decameride,  120,  134. 
Dechales,  R.  P.  C.  F.  M.,  25,  36. 
Declaracion  de  instrumentos  musicales.    See 

J.  Bermudo. 
Delannoy,  58. 
Delia    imperfettioni    della   moderna    musica. 

See  G.  M.  Artusi. 
Delezenne,  C.  E.  J.,  58,  120. 
Denis,  J.,  47. 
Descartes,  R.,  53. 
U  desiderio.    See  E.  Bottrigari. 
deviation,  iii  and  passim. 
Dialogo  della  musica  antica  e  moderna.    See 

V.  Galilei, 
diaschismata,  121. 
diatonic  genus.    See  Greek  tunings. 
Dictionnaire  de  musique.  See  J.  J.  Rousseau. 
Didymus,  2,  18,  20  f,  23,  887109,  152,  195. 
diesis,  10,  108,  196. 

Dimostrationi  armoniche.    See  G.  Zarlino. 
Discorsi  ...  e  due  nuove  scienze.    See  G.  Gal- 
ilei, 
ditone,  21  f,  115. 
ditonic  comma,    i  and  passim.    See  especially 

irregular    systems:    divisions    of    ditonic 

comma, 
division,    arithmetical,    21,  29  f,  56  f,  60-64, 

68-73,  80,  83,  85,  99,   139-144,  150,    155, 

162,  175. 
division,  geometrical,  38  f,  80,  92  f,  150,  154. 

See  also  Euclidean  construction. 
Don  Giovanni.   See  W.  A.  Mozart. 


Doni,  G.  B.,  109  f. 

Douwes,  C,  30,  146. 

Dowland,  J.,  4,  24,  151,  153. 

Dowland,  R.,  151. 

Drobisch,  M.  W.,  37,  114,  120,  123,  125,  127- 

129. 
dulcimer,  112,  190. 
duplication  of  the  cube,  49. 
Dupont,  W.,  91,  105. 
Durham  Cathedral,  106. 

"Eine  mathematisch-harmonische  Analyse  des 

Don  Giovanni  von  Mozart."    See  G.  Engel. 
Eitz,  K.  A.  ii. 

Elementa  musica.    See  Q.  van  Blankenburg. 
Elementa  musicalis.    See  Faber  Stapulensis. 
Elementarbuch    der     Tonkunst.     See    H.    P. 

Bossier. 
An  Elementary  Treatise  on  Musical  Intervals 

and  Temperament.  See  R.  H.  M.  Bosanquet. 
Elements    of   Musical    Composition.    See   W. 

Crotch. 
An    Elizabethan   Virginal    Book.    See    E.    W. 

Naylor. 
Ellis,  A.  J.,  ii,  48  f,  65,  73-76,  87,  111,  131, 

146,  193-195,  198  f. 
Elsasz,  113. 
Engel,  G.,  196. 

enharmonic  genus.    See  Greek  tunings. 
Enharmonic  Organ,  110  f. 
Enharmonium,  111. 
eptameride,  120. 

equal  temperament.    See  temperament,  equal. 
Eratosthenes,  16,  18  f,  22  f. 
Erlangen  University  Library,  91. 
Escuela  musica.    See  P.  Nassarre. 
espinette,  47. 
An  Essay  upon  Perfect  Intonation.  See  H.  Lis- 

ton. 
Euclidean  construction,  27,  50,  52-55,  93,  137, 

142,  144. 
Euharmonic  Organ,  110. 
Euler,  L.,  65,  100  f,  109  f. 
Eutocius,  50. 
exponents,  ii,  95,  102. 
Exposition  de  quelques  nouvelles  vues  mathe- 

matiques   dans  la   theorie  de    la  musique, 

125. 
Exposition  d'une  nouvelle  methode  pour  l'en- 

seignement  de  la  musique.    See  P.  Galin. 
extensions  of  just  intonation.    See  multiple  di- 
vision:   extensions  of  just  intonation. 
Extract  aus  der  neuen  Musica  oder  Singkunst. 

See  D.  Hizler. 

Faber   Stapulensis  (Jacques  le  Febvre),  137, 

144. 
Faggott,  J.  65-67. 


220 


INDEX 


Farey,  J.,  65,  134. 

Farnaby,  G.,  188. 

Faulhaber,  J.,  78,  80. 

Febvre,  J.  le.    See  Faber  Stapulensis. 

Fetis,  F.  J.,  114. 

Fibonacci  series,  115. 

Fischer,  J.  P.  A.,  7. 

Fitzwilliam  Virginal  Book,  188. 

Fludd,  R.,  Frontispiece,  3. 

flute.    See  wind  instruments. 

Fogliano,  L.,  11,  92-96,  104,  106. 

Foundling  Hospital,  106. 

"Frequency  Ratios  of  the  Tempered  Scale." 
See  C.  Williamson. 

Frescobaldi,  G.,  189,  192. 

fretted  clavichord.    See  clavichord,  bonded. 

fretted  instruments,  6-8,  11,  25,  28,  40,  42, 
45  f,  50,  57-59,  98  f,  103, 139-142,  144-149, 
151,  162-164,  182-186,  188,  197. 

fretted  instruments,  in  sixteenth  century  paint- 
ings, 12. 

Fritz,  B.,  47  f. 

Froberger,  J.  J.,  190. 

Fronimo.    See  V.  Galilei. 

Fuller-Maitland,  J.  A.,  188. 

Gafurius,  F.,  3,  5,  25. 

Galilei,  G.,  11. 

Galilei,  V.,  8,  46,  57-64,  149,  184. 

Galin,  P.,  31. 

Galle  (J.  B.  Gallet),  79,  81,  84. 

Gallimard,  J.  E.,  12,  118,  134  f,  137  f. 

Ganassi,  S.,  10,  24,  68,  70,  139-143,  154,  176, 
186. 

Gantzlich  erschopfte,  mathematische  Abthei- 
lungen  des  diatonisch-chromatischen,  tem- 
perirten  Canonis  Monochordi.  See  3.  G. 
Neidhardt. 

Garnault,  P.,  58  f. 

Gedanken  iiber  die  Temperatur  des  Herrn 
Kirnberger.    See  G.  F.  Tempelhof. 

generalized  keyboard,  9,  117,  130. 

G^ne'ration  harmonique.    See  J.  P.  Rameau. 

geometrical  approximations.  See  tempera- 
ment, equal:  geometrical  and  mechanical 
approximations. 

geometrical  division.  See  division,  geometri- 
cal. 

Geschichte  der  Instrumentalmusik  im  16. 
Jahrhundert.    See  J.  W.  Wasielewski. 

Geschichte  der  musicalischen  Temperatur. 
See  W.  Dupont. 

Geschichte  der  Musiktheorie.  See  H.  Riemann. 

Gesprach  zwischen  einem  Musico  theoretico 
und  einem  Studioso  musices.  See  G.  A. 
Sorge. 

Gesualdo,  C.,  197. 

Gibbons,  O.,  187,  189,  192. 


Gibelius,  O.,  29,  85. 

Gintzler,  S.,  185. 

Glyn,  M.  H.,  186. 

Goetschius,  P.,  4. 

golden  system,  127. 

Das  goldene  Tonsystem  ...  .  See  T.  Kornerup. 

Gonzaga,  117. 

"good"  temperaments.  See  irregular  systems: 

circulating  temperaments. 
Gow,  J.,  50  f. 
Grammateus,  H.,  3,  6,  10,  13,  25,  46,  69,  137- 

139,  142,  144,  151,  157,  176,  186. 
Greek  tunings,  15-24. 

diatonic,  15,  19-21. 

chromatic,  15,  17  f,  21. 

enharmonic,  15  f,  21,  109,  115. 

enharmonic,  modern,  15,33  f,  188,  190,  193, 
198. 

enharmonic  of  Salinas,  106  f. 
Greene,  P.  C.,  195. 
Grondig  Ondersoek  van  de  Toonen  der  Musijk. 

See  C.  Douwes. 
Guido  of  Arezzo,  25,  36. 
guitar.    See  fretted  instruments. 

hackebort.    See  dulcimer. 

Haser,  A.  fTTTOI. 

Hammond,  L.,  74-76. 

Hammond  Electric  Organ,  74-76,  199. 

Handel,  G.  F.,  10,  106. 

Harmonices  mundi.    See  J.  Kepler. 

Harmonicorum  libri  xn.    See  M.  Mersenne. 

Harmonicorum  libri  tres.  See  C.  Ptolemy. 

Harmonics.    See  R.  Smith. 

Harmonie  universelle.    See  M.  Mersenne. 

harmonium.    See  keyboard  instruments. 

harp,  198  f. 

harpsichord.    See  keyboard  instruments. 

Harrison,  40  f. 

Hawkes,  W.,  133  f,  137  f. 

Helmholtz,  H.  L.  F.,  48,  73  f ,  89,  109  f. 

Henfling,  K.,  120. 

Hero  (Heron)  of  Alexandria,  53. 

Herschel,  J.,  129. 

Hindoo   scale.    See    multiple    division:    equal 

divisions:    22-division. 
Histoire  des  mathe'matiques.    See  J.  E.  Mon- 

tucla. 
Histoire   generate   de  la  musique.    See  F.  J. 

Fetis. 
Hizler,  D.,  118. 
H&  Tchheng-thyen,  55  f,  150. 
Holbein,  H.,  12. 
Holder,  W.,  47,  123. 
Hugo  de  Reutlingen,  88. 
Hutton,  C.,  50. 

Huyghens,  C.,  9,  37,  116-118. 
Hypomnemata  musica.  See  A.  Werckmeister. 


221 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Ingenieurschul.    See  J.  Faulhaber. 

inharmonic  ity  of  upper  partials,  199. 

instruments.  See  fretted  instruments,  key- 
board instruments,  stringed  instruments, 
wind  instruments;  also,  bells,  dulcimer, 
harp. 

"Intonation  of  Solo  and  Ensemble  Perfor- 
mance."   See  J.  F.  Nickerson. 

"Introduzione  a  una  nuova  teoria  di  musica." 
See  A.  Barca. 

irregular  systems,  4,  6,  10,  24,  32,  131-182, 
200. 

irregular  systems:  circulating  temperaments: 
12  f ,  155,  165,  168,  176-182,  192  f. 

irregular  systems:  circulating  temperaments: 
Out-of-Tune  Piano,  161. 

irregular  systems:  circulating  temperaments: 
temperament  by  regularly  varied  fifths,  iii, 
179-182. 

irregular  systems:  divisions  of  ditonic  com- 
ma, 12,  44,  84,  154-175.  1/2  comma,  138, 
155-157;  1/3,  157-160;  ~tj\,  159  f;  1/5, 
159-161;  1767  161-164,  170,  179;  1/6,  l/i, 
171-173;  177,  164;  1/12,  1/4,  171  f;  1/12, 
1/6,  136,  165-172,  178  f;  1/12,  1/6,  1/4", 
173-175;  1/12,  3/16,  166;  1/12,  5/24,  170  f. 

irregular  system:    Metius'  system,  175  f. 

irregular  systems:  modifications  of  regular 
temperaments,  24,  137-149. 

irregular  systems:  modifications  of  regular 
temperaments:  Pythagorean,  46,  137  f, 
186. 

irregular  systems:  modifications  of  regular 
temperaments:    just,  139-144,  186. 

irregular  systems:  modifications  of  regular 
temperaments:  meantone,  12,  46,  131-135, 
144-149,  179,  186. 

irregular  systems:  temperaments  largely 
Pythagorean,  12,  61,  149-154. 

"Irregular  Systems  of  Temperament."  See 
J.  M.  Barbour. 

Istitutioni  armonische.    See  G.  Zarlino. 

Jackson,  W.,  125. 

Jacobi,  K.  G.  J.,  129. 

Jank6,  P.  von,  120. 

Jeans,  J.,  4. 

"Jenseits  von  Temperierung  und  Tonalitat." 
See  A.  M.  Awraamoff. 

Johann  Sebastian  Bach.   See  P.  Spitta. 

Judenkunig,  H.,  185  f. 

just  intonation,  2,  4,  9-11,  21,  88-104,  121, 
131,  176,  199. 

just  intonation,  extensions  of.  See  multiple 
division:    extensions  of  just  intonation. 

just  intonation  in  choral  music,  194-197. 

just  intonation,  modifications  of.  See  irregu- 
lar systems:  modifications  of  regular 
temperaments. 


just  intonation,  theory  of,  101-104. 

"Just  Intonation  Confuted."  See  J.  M.  Barbour. 

Keller,  G.,  47. 

Kepler,  J.,  11,  58-60,  96  f,  99. 

keyboard  instruments,  6-9,  25-48,  61,  89, 
97-99,  103,  105-123,  130,  135  f,  139,  142, 
147-149,186-195,197-199.  See  also  abacus 
Triharmonicus,  Archicembalo,  Archicym- 
balam,  Enharmonic  Organ,  Enharmonium, 
Euharmonic  Organ,  Hammond  Electric  Or- 
gan, Pantalonisches  Cimbal,  Telharmoni- 
um. 

King  FSng;  122. 

Kinkeldey,  O.,  iv,  26,  45,  186. 

Kircher,  A.,  9,  52  f,  108,  115,  122-124. 

Kirnberger,  J.  P.,  12,  64  f,  155-157,  163. 

Kbrte,  O.,  183. 

Klangeintheilungs-,  Stimmungs-  und  Temper- 
atur-Lehre.    See  C.  L.  G.  von  Wiese. 

Kornerup,  T.,  113,  121,  127  f,  195. 

Kroyer,  T.,  197. 

Kuhnau,  J.,  190  f. 

Die  Kunst  des  reinen  Satzes  in  der  Musik. 
See  J.  P.  Kirnberger. 

Kurze  Anflihrung  zum  General- Bass.  See  J. 
H.  Buttstett. 

La  Laurencie,  L.  de,  71. 

Lambert,  J.  H.,  65. 

Lanfranco,  G.  M.,   11,  45  f,  186. 

"Laute  und  Lautenmusik  bis  zur  Mitte  des  16. 

Jahrhunderts."    See  O.  Korte. 
Levens, 143-145. 
least  squares,  127  f. 

linear  correction.  See  division,  arithmetical. 
Liston,  H.,  110  f. 
Lloyd,  LI.  S.,  198. 
Lobkowitz.   See  J.  Caramuel. 
logarithms,  3,  9,  30,  41,  64,  72  f,  77-79,  116, 

118,  120,  128  f,  134,  154,  176. 
lute.    See  fretted  instruments. 

madrigal,  144,  196  f. 

Malcolm,  A.,  3,  99  f,  141,  143. 

Manuel  du  luthier.  See  J.  C.  Robet-Maugin. 

Marchettus  of  Padua,  118. 

Marenzio,  L.,  144,  197. 

Marinati,  A.,  45  f. 

Marpurg,  F.  W.,  ii,  12,  43  f,  53,  65,  68,  84, 
88,  98-100,  103  f,  139,  143,  154-182  (pas- 
sim). 

Mathematical  Dictionary.    See  C.  Hutton. 

Mathesis  nova.    See  J.  Caramuel. 

Mattheson,  J.,  124,  192. 

Marziale,  M.,  12. 

mean  proportional.  See  Euclidean  construc- 
tion. 


222 


INDEX 


mean-semitone  temperament.  See  irregular 
systems:  modifications  of  regular  temper- 
aments. 

meantone  temperament.  See  temperament, 
meantone. 

mechanical  approximations.  See  tempera- 
ment, equal:  geometrical  and  mechanical 
approximations. 

Meckenheuser,  J.  G.,  80,  83. 

Melder,  J.,  78. 

"Memoire  sur  les  valeurs  numeriques  des 
notes  de  la  gamme."  See  C.  E.  J.  Dele- 
zenne. 

"Memoire  th£orique  &  pratique  sur  les  sys- 
temes  tempe're's  de  musique."  See  J.  B. 
Romieu. 

Mercadier,  J.  B.,  135,  166  f. 

Mercator,  N.,  9,  118,  123. 

Merian,  W.,  187. 

meride,  120. 

Mersenne,  M.,  iii,  7,  9,  11  f,  48,  51-55,  58  f, 
61,  74,  79  f,  84,  87,  97-99,  103,  106-108, 
118  f,  122,  132-134,  137,  183,  190. 

Merula,  T.,  187,  189. 

mesolabium,  6,  33,  50  f,  59,  118  f,  144. 

Metius,  A.,  175  f,  181. 

Micrologus.    See  A.  Ornithoparchus. 

Miller,  D.  C.,~85. 

Molth^e,  52. 

monochord,  passim. 

Monochordum.    See  A.  Reinhard. 

monopipe,  85-87. 

Monteverdi,  C.,  8. 

Montucla,  J.  E-,  89. 

Montvallon,  A.  B.  de,  100  f . 

Morley,  T.,  151. 

Mozart,  W.  A.,  196. 

multiple  division,  105-130.  See  also  split 
keys. 

multiple  division:  equal  divisions,  111-130; 
17-division,  112,  120,  123,  126,  128;  19-, 
9,  34,  112-114,  120  f,  127,  129  f;  22-,  114  f, 
119,  128-130;  24-,  115;  25-,  129;  28-,  129; 
29-,  115,  120,  125  f,  128;  3L-,  9,  31,  36  f, 
42,  51,  115-121,  127-129,  152;  34-,  37,  119, 
129;  36-,  119;  41-,  119  f,  126,  128  f;  43-, 
36,  118,  120,  123,  126,  128;  46-,  128;  50-, 
33,  41  f,  120  f,  127;  51-,  128;  53-,  9,  Tl8, 
120-123,  125  f,  128-130;  55-,  42  f,  120, 
122-126,  191;  56-,  119,  125  f;  58-,  125; 
.  65-,  125  f,  128;  67-,  122,  125;  70-,  126, 
128;  74-,  37,  119,  125,  128;  77-,  126,  128; 
79-,  125;  81-,  127;  84-,  125;  87^,  119,  125  f, 
129;  89-,  126,  128;  9L-,  125;  94-,  126,  128; 
98-,  125;  105-,  125;  112-,  125;  117-,  125; 
118-,  125-129;  131-,  127;  142-,  128;  166-, 
128;  212-,  127;  306-,  128;  343-,  127;  559-, 
129;  612-,  129;  665-,  128  f;  817-,  129. 

multiple    division:    equal    divisions:    corres- 


pondences with  varieties  of  meantone  tem- 
perament, 124. 

multiple  division:  extensions  of  just  intona- 
tion, 79,  106-112. 

multiple  division,  theory  of,  126-130. 

"Music  and  Ternary  Continued  Fractions." 
See  J.  M.  Barbour. 

Musica  instrumentalis  deudsch.  See  M.  Agri- 
cola. 

Musica  mathematica.    See  A.  Bartolus. 

Musica  mechanica  organoedi.    See  J.  Adlung. 


Musica  practica 
Musica  theorica 


See  B.  Ramis. 
See  L.  Fogliano. 

Musicae  activae  Micrologus.    See  A.  Ornitho- 
parchus. 

"Musical  Logarithma."    See  J.  M.  Barbour. 

Musicalische    Paradoxal-Discourse.    See    A. 
Werckmeister. 

Musicalische    Temperatur.     See   A.    Werck- 
meister. 

La  musique  rendue  sensible  par  la  mechan- 
ique.    See  H.  L.  Choquel. 

Musurgia  universalis.    See  A.  Kircher. 

"The  Myth  of  Equal  Temperament."    See  LI. 
S.  Lloyd. 

Nassarre,  P.,  61-64. 

National  Gallery  in  London,  12. 

"Die   natiirliche   Stimmung   in   der  modernen 

Vokalmusik."    See  M.  Planck. 
Naylor,  E.  W.,  188- 
negative  system,  112-125  (passim). 
Neidhardt,  J.  G.,  ii,  12,  44,  78,  80,  82,  85-87, 

115,  118,  154-182  (passim),  192  f,  200. 
New  Cyclopedia.   See  A.  Rees. 
Ayn  new  kunstlich  Buech.  See  H.  Grammateus. 
"A  New   Theory  of  Untempered  Music."    See 

N.  L.  Norden. 
Newton,  I.,  53. 
Nickerson,  J.  F.,  195. 
Nicomedes,  53. 
Nierop,  D.  R.  van,  175. 
"Nierop's  Hackebort."    See  dulcimer. 
Norden,  N.  L.,  195. 
Nouveau  systeme  de  musique  ...  .    See  A.  B. 

Montvallon. 
Nouveau  systeme  de  musique  theorique.    See 

J.  P.  Rameau. 
Nouveau    systeme   de    musique   theorique    et 

pratique.    See  J.  B.  Mercadier. 
Nova  &  exquisita  Monochordi  Dimensio.    See 

C.  Schneegass. 
"Novus  cyclus  harmonicus."  See  C.Huyghens. 
numerical  approximations.    See  temperament, 

equal:    numerical    approximations;      also, 

temperament,  meantone:    approximations. 
"Nytt    Pifund,    til   at   finna   Temperaturen,    i 

stamningen  for   thonerne   pa    Claveretock 

dylika  Instrumenter."    See  D.  P.  Strahle. 


223 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


"Observations    on    the    Vibrations    of    Piano 
Strings."  See  O.  H.  Schuck  andR.  W.  Young. 

Odington,  W.,  3. 

Oettingen,  A.  von,  119. 

omega  (o),  127. 

"On  a   New  Mode  of   Equally  Tempering   the 
Musical  Scale."    See  J.  Farey. 

"On  music."    See  J.  Farey. 

"On  Perfect  Harmony  in  Music  ...  ."    See  H. 
W.  Poole. 

"On    Perfect  Musical  Intonation."    See  H.  W. 
Poole. 

"On  the  History  of  Musical  Pitch."    See  A.  J. 
Ellis. 

"On  the  Musical  Scales  of  Various  Nations." 
See  A.  J.  Ellis. 

On  the  Principles  and  Practice  of  Just  Intona- 
tion.   See  P.  Thompson. 

Opelt,  F.  W.,  99,  114,  119  f. 

Orfeo.    See  C.  Monteverdi. 

See  keyboard  instruments. 
See  J.  P.  Bendeler. 


organ. 

Organopoeia. 

Orgelund  Klavier  in  der  Musik  des  16. 


Jahr- 


hunderts.    See  O.  Kinkeldey. 

Ornithoparchus,  A.,  3,  151. 

"Other  Necessary  Observations  to  Lute-Play- 
ing."   See  J.  Dow  land. 

"Outlines  of  Experiments  and  Inquiries  Re- 
specting Sound  and  Light."    See  T.  Young. 

Out-of-Tune  Piano.  See  irregular  systems: 
circulating  temperaments:  Out-of-Tune 
Piano. 

Oystermayre,  J.,  188. 

Pachelbel,  J.,  190,  193. 

paintings  of  the  sixteenth  century,  12. 

Palestrina,  G.  P.  da,  196. 

Pantalonisches  Cimbal,  190. 

Papius,  A.,  3. 

Pappius  of  Alexandria,  50. 

parfait  diapason  of  Mersenne,  106-108. 

Parry,  H.,  192. 

Parthenia,  187. 

"The  Persistence  of  the  Pythagorean  Tuning 

System."    See  J.  M.  Barbour. 
Pesarese,  D.,  33,  113. 
Philander,  W.,  118. 
Philo  of  Byzantium,  51. 
Philolaus,  121. 
The    Philosophy   of   Musical  Sounds.   See    R. 

Smith. 
Phrynis  Mytilenaeus  ...  .    See  W.C.  Printz. 
pi  (it),  40  f,  77. 

piano.    See  keyboard  instruments. 
Piano    Tuning   and    Allied   Arts.    See   W.    B. 

White. 
A  Plaine  and  Easie  Introduction  to  Practicall 

Musicke.    See  T.  Morley. 


Planck,  M.,  11. 

Plato,  53. 

Poole,  H.  W.,  110. 

Populare  Darstellung  der  Akustik.  See  H. 
Riemann. 

positive  system,  112-125  (passim). 

Practica  musica.    See  F.  Gafurius. 

Praetorius,  M.,  9,  28  f,  62,  113. 

Prattica  di  musica.    See  L.  Zacconi. 

Precis  d'acoustique  ....    See  J.  P.  L.  Anglas. 

Predis,  A.  de,  12. 

Prelleur,  P.,  47. 

present  practice  of  tuning,  197-200. 

"Present-Day  Preferences  for  Certain  Mel- 
odic Intervals."    See  A.  M.  Small. 

"Principles  of  the  Science  of  Tuning  Instru- 
ments with  Fixed  Tones."  See  C.  Stanhope. 

Printz,  W.  C,  29,  37,  119,  142  f. 

Pro  clavichordiis  faciendis,  91  f. 

Propositiones  mathematico-musicae.  See  O. 
Gibelius. 

Prout,  E.,  4. 

Ptolemy,  C.,  2,  16-23,  57,  88,  152,  195. 

Pythagoras,  1,  139. 

Pythagorean  tuning,  1-4,  10,  21-23,  42,  45,  56, 
59,  68,  88-91,  95,  101  f,  110-112, 121  f,  131, 
147,  150  f,  176,  183,  194,  198. 

Pythagorean  tuning,  modifications  of.  See  ir- 
regular systems:  modifications  of  regular 
temperaments,  and  temperaments  largely 
Pythagorean. 

Railsback,  O.  L.,  199. 

Les  raisons  des  forces  mouvantes  avec  di- 
ver ses  machines.    See  S.  de  Caus. 

Ramarin,  124. 

Rameau,  J.  P.,  4,  11  f,  133,  137,  192. 

Ramis,  B.,  4,  10,  25,  88-92,  104  f,  151,  194. 

Redford,  J.,  186  f. 

Rees,  A.,  65. 

Regola  facile  e  breve  per  sonare  sopra  il 
basso  continuo.    See  G.  Sabbatini. 

Regola  Rubertina.    See  S.  Ganassi. 

regular  temperament.  See  temperament,  reg- 
ular. 

Reinhard,  A.,  68,  141-143,  186. 

Das  Relativitatsprincip  der  muslkalischen 
Harmonie.    See  Ariel. 

"Remarques  sur  les  temperaments  en  mu- 
sique."    See  J.  H.  Lambert. 

Riemann,  H.,  25,  114,  116  f,  119  f,  135. 

Roberti,  E.  de,  12. 

Roberval,  52. 

Robet-Maugin,  J.  C,  58. 

Romberg,  B.,  58  f. 

Romieu,  J.  B.,  37,  40,  42  f,  101,  114,  119  f, 
123-126. 

Rore,  C.  da,  196  f. 


224 


INDEX 


Rossi,  L.,  29  f,  35  f,  51,  53,  115,  118-120. 

Rossi,  M.,  187  f. 

Rousseau,  J.,  105. 

Rousseau,  J.  J.,  99  f,  104. 

Roussier,  P.  J.,  4. 

Rudimenta  musices.    See  M.  Agricola. 

Ruscelli,  G.,  6. 

Sabbatini,  G.,  108. 

Sachs,  C.,  iii,  197. 

St.  Martin's  Church  in  Lucca,  105  f. 
Salinas,    F.,  6,  9,  33-35,  42,  46,  50  f,   106  f, 
113,  118,  186. 

Salmon,  T.,  143. 

Sambuca  Lincea,  151-154. 

Sancta  Maria,  Tomas  de,  28. 

Per  satyrische  Componist.  See  W.C.  Printz. 

Sauveur,  J.,  112,  114,  118,  120,  123-126,  129. 

scale,  Arabian.  See  multiple  division:  equal 
divisions:    17-. 

scale,  Hindoo.  See  multiple  division:  equal 
divisions:    22-. 

scale,  Siamese,  112. 

Scarlatti,  D.,  191-193. 

Scheidt,  S.,  189. 

A  Scheme  Demonstrating  the  Perfection  and 
Harmony  of  Sounds.    See  W.  Jackson. 

Schering,  A.,  186. 

schisma,  64,  80,  89,  92,  110  f,  154,  156. 

Schlick,  A.,  6,  10,  26,  46,  131,  135-139,  168, 
181  f,  186  f. 

Schneegass,  C.,  37-40,  119. 

Schbnberg,  A.,  114,  183. 

Schola  phonologica.    See  J.  Beer. 

Schreyber,  H.    See  Grammateus. 

Schroter,  C.  G.,  68-73,  77. 

Schuck,  O.  H.,  199. 

Schumann,  K.  E.,  159. 

Science  and  Music.    See  J.  Jeans. 

Scintille  de  musica.   See  G.  M.  Lanfranco. 

Scriabin,  A.,  113. 

Seashore,  C.  E.,  196. 

Seconda  parte  dell'  Artusi.  See  G.  M.  Artusi. 

Sectio  Canonis  harmonici.    See  J.  G.  Neidhardt. 

semi-meantone  temperament.  See  irregular 
systems:  modifications  of  regular  tem- 
peraments:   mean-semitone  temperament. 

"Ein  Sendschreiben  liber  Temperatur-Berech- 
nung."    See  C.  G.  Schroter. 

Senfl,  L.,  1857 

Sensations  of  Tone.  See  H.  L.  F.  Helmholtz; 
also,  A.  J.  Ellis. 

Septenarium  temperament,  164. 

Serini,  G.,  191. 

sesqui-.    See  superparticular  ratio. 

Seu-ma  Pyeou,  122. 

sexagesimal  notation,  16,  79-81. 

A  Short  History  of  Greek  Mathematics.  See 
J.  Gow. 


Siamese  scale,  112. 

Silbermann,  G.,  9,  13,  42,  112,  124,  191,  194. 

Sistema  musico.    See  L.  Rossi. 

"A  Sixteenth   Century   Approximation  fortr." 

See  J.  M.  Barbour. 
Small;  A.  M.,  iv,  195. 
Smith,  R.,  40-42. 

Societats-Frucht.    See  J.  G.  Meckenheuser. 
Die    sogenannte    allerneueste    musicalisches 

Temperatur.    See  J.  G.  Meckenheuser. 
Somma  de  tutte  le  scienza.    See  A.  Marinati. 
Sophiae  cum  moria  certamen.  See  R.  Fludd. 
Sopplementi  musicali.    See  G.  Zarlino. 
Sorge,  G.  A.,  42,  83  f,  1247  159,  194. 
Spataro,  G.,  4. 
"Specimen   de  novo    suo  systemate    musico." 

See  K.  Henfling. 
Spiegel  der  Orgelmacher  und  Organisten.    See 

A.  Schlick. 
spinet.    See  keyboard  instruments. 
Spitta,  P.,  85. 

split  keys,  33-35,  42,  97,  105  f. 
square  root.    See  Euclidean  construction. 
Squire,  W.  B.,~188. 
Stanhope,  C,  Earl,  157  f,  163. 
Steiner,  J.,  111. 
Stella,  S.,  117. 

Stevin,  S.,  7,  11,  28,  76  f,  79,  192. 
Str'ahle,  D.  P.,  65-68. 
stretched  octaves,  199. 
stringed  instruments,  4,  8,  45  f,  58  f,  124,  195, 

198  f. 
"Studien   im  Gebiete   der  reinen   Stimmung." 

See  S.  Tanaka. 
Sulzer,  J.  G.,  65. 
superparticular  division,  2.  See  also  Intervals 

with  Superparticular  Ratios,  the  table  fol- 
lowing this  index, 
superparticular   division,    of   the   tetrachord, 

23  f. 
superparticular  division,  of  the  tone,  154. 
Suremain-Missery,  A.,  166. 
Sweelinck,  J.  P.,  189. 
symmetry,  155-182  (passim). 
Syntagma  musicum.    See  M.  Praetorius. 
syntonic  comma,  i  and  passim. 
"Systeme  general  des  intervalles  des  sons." 

See  J.  Sauveur. 

"Table   general   des    syst^mes    tempeVe's    de 

musique.    See  J.  Sauveur. 
tabular  differences,  68-73. 
Tagore,  S.  M.,  114. 
Tallis,  T.,  186  f. 

Tanaka,  S.,  6,  33,  109,  111,  117,  122  f,  135  f. 
Der  Tanz  in  den  deutschen  Tabulaturblichern. 

See  W.  Merian. 
Telemann,  G.  P.,  124,  191  f. 
Telharmonium,  74. 


225 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


temperament,  5.    See  also  tunings. 

temperament,  by  regularly  varied  fifths.  See 
irregular  systems:  temperament  by  regu- 
larly varied  fifths. 

temperament,  circulating.  See  irregular  sys- 
tems:   circulating  temperaments. 

temperament,  equal,  6-8,  10  f,  25,  29,  45-87, 
90,  131,  142,  146  f,  164,  176-178,  183-186, 
188,  191-195,  197-200. 

temperament,  equal:  geometrical  and  mechan- 
ical approximations,  49-55. 

temperament,  equal:  numerical  approxima- 
tions, 55-87. 

temperament,  history  of,  1-14. 

temperament,  meantone,  9-11,  25-44,  71,  106, 
115,  124,  142,  176,  189-192,  197,  200. 

temperament,  meantone:  approximations, 
29-31,  43. 

temperament,  meantone:  modifications.  See 
irregular  systems:  modifications  of  regu- 
lar temperaments. 

temperament,  meantone,  varieties  of,  31-44, 
131;  1/3  comma,  9,  33-35,  51,  113;  1/5,  iii, 
35  f,  47,  120,  134;  1/6,  42  f,  112,  124,  135, 
146,  168,  176,  191;  1/7,  43;  1/8,  43;  1/9, 
44;  1/10,  44;  2/7,  9,  32  f,  35,  37,  46,  50, 
119,  121;  2/9,  36  f,  119;  3/10,  40  f;  5/18, 
41. 

temperament,  meantone,  varieties  of:  corres- 
pondences with  equal  multiple  divisions, 
124. 

temperament,  "paper,"  149,  154,  176. 

temperament,  regular,  32t44,  91,  112-131. 

Le  temperament.    See  P.  Garnault. 

temperament  anacritique,  124. 

"Temperament;  or,  the  Division  of  the  Octave." 
See  R.  H.  M.  Bosanquet. 

Tempelhof,  G.  F.,  155. 

Temple  Church  in  London,  106. 

Tentamen  novae  theoriae  musicae.  See  L. 
Euler. 

Theatri  machinarum.    See  H.  Zeisung. 

Th^orie  acoustico-musicale.  See  A.  Sure- 
main-Missery. 

A  Theory  of  Evolving  Tonality.  See  J.  Yasser. 

Thompson,  P.,  110. 

Tisdall,  W.,  188. 

Tomkins,  T.,  188. 

The  Tonometer.    See  A.  Warren. 

Das  Tonsystem  des  Italieners  Zarlino.  See 
T.  Kornerup. 

Torchi,  L.,  186. 

Toscanello  in  musica.    See  P.  Aron. 

Trait6de  l'accord  de  l'espinette.  See  J.  Denis. 

Traits  de  l'harmonie.    See  J.  P.  Rameau. 

Traite  de  la  viole.    See  J.  Rousseau. 

Transponir-Harmonium,  111. 

Treatise  ...  of  Harmony.    See  W.  Holder. 


A  Treatise  of  Musick.    See  A.  Malcolm. 

trigonometry,  65-67. 

trombone.    See  wind  instruments. 

Tsai-yii,  Prince  Chu,  7,  77-79. 

tuning,  history  of,  1-14. 

tuning  pipe,  85-87. 

tuning  today.    See  present  practice  of  tuning. 

tunings.    See    Greek   tunings,  just    intonation, 

Pythagorean  tuning,  etc. 
Two  Thousand  Years  of  Music.   See  C.  Sachs. 

"liber  mehr  als  zwolfstufigegleichschwebende 
Temperaturen."    See  P.  von  Jank6. 

"Uber  musikalisches  Tonbestimmung  und 
Temperatur."    See  M.  W.  Drobisch. 

"liber  wissenschafliche  Begrundung  derMusik 
durch  Akustik."    See  A.  F.  Haser. 

unequal  temperament.  See  temperament, 
meantone,  and  temperament,  meantone, 
varieties  of.    See  also  irregular  systems. 

Van  de  Spiegeling  der  Singconst.  See  S.  Stevin. 

Varella,  D.,  58. 

Variety  of  Lute- Lessons.    See  R.  Dowland. 

varieties  of  meantone  temperament.  See  tem- 
perament, meantone,  varieties  of. 

Verhandlung  van  de  Klokken  en  het  Klokke- 
Spel.    See  J.  P.  A.  Fischer. 

Verheijen,  A.,  28,  35,  120. 

Versuch  liber  die  musikalische  Temperatur. 
See  F.  W.  Marpurg. 

The  Vibrato.    See  C.  E.  Seashore. 

Vicentino,  N.,  8,  11,  25,  27,  37,  51,  115-119, 
142,  152,  183. 

vihuela.    See  fretted  instruments. 

viol.    See  fretted  instruments. 

violin.    See  stringed  instruments. 

"Violin  Intonation."    See  P.  C.  Greene. 

Le  violon  de  Lully~a~Viotti.  See  L.  de  La 
Laurencie. 

Violoncell  Schull.    See  B.  Romberg. 

violoncello.    See  stringed  instruments. 

virginals.    See  keyboard  instruments. 

Vitruvius,  M.,  50  f. 

voices,  124,  144,  194-197. 

Wang  Pho,  150  f. 

Warren,  A.,  118. 

Wasieleswki,  J.  W.  von,  186. 

Wedell,  P.  S.,  127. 

Werckmeister,  A.,  12  f,  105,  154-182  (passim) 

192  f. 
White,  W.  B.,  48  f. 
Wiese,  C.  L.  G.,  Baron  von,  156  f. 
Williamson,  C.,  74-76. 
wind  instruments,  7,  124,  195,  198. 
Wis-konstige  Musyka.    See  D.  R.  van  Nierop. 
wolf,  10  f,  27,  34,  91,  131  f,  134,  163. 


226 


INDEX 


Yasser,  J.,  9,  113  f. 

Young,  R.  W.,  199. 

Young,  T.,  12  f,  135,  161,  165  f,  178  f,  181  f. 

Zacconi,  L.,  46. 


Zarlino,  G.,  6,  9,  11,  25,  27,  32,  35,  37,  42, 
46,  50  f,  59,  113,  118,  121,  144,  147,  164, 
194  f. 

Zeisung,  H.,  142. 

Zipoli,  D.,  191. 


Intervals  with  i 

Juperpartici 

ilar  Ratios 

Ratios 

Intervals 

Cents 

Page  References  in  Text 

2:1 

octave 

1200 

passim 

3:2 

perfect  5th 

702 

passim 

4:3 

perfect  4th 

498 

passim 

5:4 

major  3rd 

386 

passim 

6:5 

minor  3rd 

316 

passim 

7:6 

minor  3rd 

267 

18,  19  (Table  13),  22  f,  30  f. 

8:7 

maximum  tone 

234 

19  (Table  13),  20,  23  f,  152. 

9:8 

major  tone 

204 

passim 

10:9 

minor  tone 

182 

passim 

11:10 

minimum  tone 

165 

21  f,  154. 

12:11 

semitone 

150 

18,  21,  152-154. 

13:12 

• 

139 

23,  154. 

14:13 
15:14 

approximation  to 

meantone  diatonic 

semitone 

128 
119 

23. 

17  (Table  5),  18,  23,  30  f,  152-1E 

16:15 

just  diatonic 
semitone 

112 

passim 

17:16 
18:17 

semitone 

approximation  to 

semitone  of  equal 

temperament 

105 
99 

57,  141,  153  f. 

8,  57-64,  141,  153  f,  186. 

19:18 

semitone 

93 

17  (Table  7),  18,  57,  141,  154. 

20:19 

• 

89 

16  (Table  2),  17  (Table  7), 

19  (Tables  13  and  14),  18,  141, 1 

21:20 

" 

84 

20,  22,  153  f. 

22:21 

• 

81 

18,  23,  151,  153. 

227 


TUNING  AND  TEMPERAMENT 


Ratios 

Intervals 
approximation  to 

Cents 

Page  References  in  Text 

24:23 

meantone  chromatic 
semitone 

74 

16,  30  f. 

25:24 

just  chromatic 
semitone 

70 

passim 

26:25 

quartertone 

68 

23. 

27:26 

- 

65 

154. 

28:27 

• 

63 

16,  18,  20,  22,  152. 

31:30 

■ 

57 

21,  23,  109. 

32:31 

- 

55 

21,  23,  109. 

33:32 

■ 

53 

152,  154. 

36:35 

- 

49 

16,  23. 

39:38 

■ 

45 

16  (Table  2). 

40:39 

" 

44 

16  (Table  2),  23,  154. 

45:44 

" 

39 

154. 

46:45 

" 

38 

16,  23. 

49:48 

" 

36 

24,  152. 

55:54 

comma 

32 

152  f. 

56:55 

■ 

31 

23. 

64:63 

approximation 

27 

23,  152. 

74:73 

to  ditonic 
comma 

24 

passim 

81:80 

syntonic 
comma 

22 

passim 

228 


M  1/ 


DATE  DUE 

■ay  a 

9   1993 

CAYLOHO 

PRINTED  IN  U.S.A. 

OCT  4 


3  5002  00368  4748 

Barbour,  James  Murray 

Tuning  and  temperament  :  a  historical  su 


ML   3809   B234 


AUTHOR 

Barbour 


TITLE 


.  -l~~     „^A 


\  r\  -v  om/nT"*  t 


Music 

ML 

3809 

B234 


274194